Working out the best deal: the role of consumer numerical skills within a grocery shop

Emma Beacom (Department of Food Business and Development, Cork University Business School, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland)
Lynsey Elizabeth Hollywood (Department of Hospitality and Tourism Management, Ulster University Business School, Ulster University, Belfast, UK)
Victoria Simms (School of Psychology, Ulster University, Coleraine, UK)
Alison Wynne (Department of Hospitality and Tourism Management, Ulster University Business School, Ulster University, Belfast, UK)

British Food Journal

ISSN: 0007-070X

Article publication date: 12 April 2022

Issue publication date: 19 December 2022

1122

Abstract

Purpose

Price promotions are a common tool used by retailers to increase sales. This study aims to investigate the effect of consumer's numerical skills and other demographic characteristics on their ability to determine the best deal when conducting a grocery shop (referred to as deal competency).

Design/methodology/approach

A consumer survey (n = 308) was conducted online, collecting information about respondent's demographics and grocery shopping behaviours, numerical literacy using the subjective numeracy scale (SNS), and deal competency (a novel measure). Multiple regression analysis and Pearson's correlations were conducted using SPSSv26.

Findings

Overall, the mean SNS score for the total sample was 31.47 (SD = 8.27), and the mean sample deal competency score was 13.5 (SD = 2.3). Spearman's correlation analysis identified a moderate significant positive relationship between numerical skills and deal competency, rs(303) = 0.360, p < 0.001. Regression analysis found significant positive relationships between numerical skills and being male, and with mathematical achievement; and between deal competency and age, mathematical achievement and educational achievement. Regarding buying behaviour, correlation analyses identified only one significant relationship between numerical skills (SNS score) and deal competency and variables relating to buying behaviour, namely a negative relationship between deal competency and amount spent on promotional food items in top up grocery shops.

Originality/value

This study contributes to the gap in literature regarding consumer ability to work out the best deal on promotions, presents a novel scale for describing consumer deal competency, and considers the comparative usefulness of using objective and subjective scales in similar studies.

Keywords

Citation

Beacom, E., Hollywood, L.E., Simms, V. and Wynne, A. (2022), "Working out the best deal: the role of consumer numerical skills within a grocery shop", British Food Journal, Vol. 124 No. 13, pp. 237-253. https://doi.org/10.1108/BFJ-10-2021-1110

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2022, Emma Beacom, Lynsey Elizabeth Hollywood, Victoria Simms and Alison Wynne

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

Price is an important factor for consumers when choosing groceries, and price promotions are a common tool used by retailers to increase sales (Carlson and Kukar-Kinney, 2020; Zorbas et al., 2020). In the context of this research, “promotions” refers to a temporary reduction in price of an item (Hollywood et al., 2016). Promotions can take a variety of forms, such as bulk discounts (e.g. buy one get one free), price reduction (e.g. save 50%), standalone offer (e.g. marked as “only £1”), multi buys (e.g. any 3 for £5), mix and match (e.g. 3 for 2 – cheapest free), extra free (e.g. 33% extra free, 150 ml extra free) and meal deals with choice (e.g. main, side and dessert for £10) (Bogomolova et al., 2015; Hollywood et al., 2016). These strategies can be useful for retailers and brands to stimulate sales as they increase a consumers' perception of value (Choi and Coulter, 2012). However, calculating cost savings requires numerical skills, and can cause some difficulty in accurately determining the cost savings (Chen and Rao, 2007; DelVecchio et al., 2007; Tan and Bogomolova, 2016; Gordon-Hecker et al., 2020).

Numerical skills aid consumers in understanding, processing and comparing differences in prices between different product options (Tan and Bogomolova, 2016; Zorbas et al., 2020) allowing them to make a more informed decision, and these skills are a particularly important consideration for consumers on low budgets (Tan and Bogomolova, 2016). However, there is wide variation in consumers’ numeracy skills, and this can impact upon judgement and decision making (Weller et al., 2013). If consumers cannot process information effectively, they are less informed and have reduced agency when responding to marketing communications, for example, when responding to point-of-sale promotional offers (Harrison-Walker, 1995) and mistakenly choosing the least valuable deal (Jae and Delvecchio, 2004). Generally, price promotions have been suggested to be potentially misleading or confusing, thus it is assumed that this confusion will be greater for those with low numerical skills (Tan and Bogomolova, 2016). From the retail management perspective, certain promotional strategies may be more effective in reducing the amount of time consumers spend considering the value of the promotional offer, thereby potentially maximising sales (Gordon-Hecker et al., 2020). Although various studies have considered the impact of numerical skills on decision making generally (e.g. Peters, 2012; Sinayev and Peters, 2015), consumer decision making in retail environments (e.g. Graffeo et al., 2015; Tan and Bogomolova, 2016), or literacy of promotions or products in the retail environments (e.g. Gau and Viswanathan, 2008; Tan and Bogomolova, 2016), there are limited studies specifically relating to consumers numerical skills and the grocery shop. This study therefore examines the influence of consumers numerical skills on their ability to determine the best deal (best value) on grocery products. Considering the grocery shop is of particular interest as this type of activity is often faster paced and more impulsive, requiring more decisions to be made during one shop (due to the number of items typically being purchased). In addition, more items are likely to be on promotional offer due to the sheer volume of SKUs in any one store compared to purchases in another type of store (e.g. electronics). This time pressure combined with the increased number of decisions to be made (and number of alternative options) may increase the likelihood of consumers incorrectly calculating the best deal (c.f. Caviola et al., 2017 on broader mathematical processing under time pressure). Therefore, this study considers consumers ability to work out the best deal (deal competency) alongside numerical skills and related consumer behaviour and demographics.

Background

There has been a large influx of promotional offers within the grocery sector, with various promotions being viewed as misleading, making it difficult for the consumer to determine the best deal (Babin et al., 2004; Garaus et al., 2015; Competition and Markets Authority, 2015). The nature of the promotion type and the related complexity of the calculation needed to evaluate the value of the promotion can impact consumers’ ability to determine the best deal, for example, percentage-off discounts have been identified as potentially more difficult for consumers to compute, and more prone to error (Chen and Rao, 2007; DelVecchio et al., 2007; Gordon-Hecker et al., 2020).

Consumers may find it difficult to determine the best grocery deal for various circumstantial and demographic reasons, such as financial and time constraints, product availability and cognitive differences regarding numerical skills (Choi and Mattila, 2014; Lee et al., 2015). Consumer ability to correctly calculate and appraise deals is particularly important for grocery (food and drink) items as these are necessary products which are bought in high volume and thus represent an ongoing significant proportion of the household budget. Budgetary constraints may be of heightened relevance to certain consumers, influencing the amount and types of food (and drink) they are able to buy, i.e. influencing the adequacy and the healthiness of their shopping basket.

In response to concerns about misleading grocery prices, in 2015 the Competition and Markets Authority (CMA) introduced unit pricing to stores in the United Kingdom (UK) to allow consumers to compare prices of groceries (CMA, 2015). Unit pricing refers to the display of the cost of products per unit of weight, or volume, to allow consumers to compare product costing (Bogomolova et al., 2020), i.e. unit pricing enables consumers to look past the promotional offer and see how much the product costs per (e.g.) 100 g, so that they can confirm the value offering of the product in comparison to alternatives. Unit pricing is useful for consumers as it can allow them to more quickly make a value related decision to complete their shopping task. For retailers, unit pricing can potentially increase customer satisfaction and improve store image (Yao and Oppewal, 2016). Unit pricing is widely used globally, but while mandated in some regions, such as the UK, the European Union, Australia and some states in the United States, it is not mandatory in all regions where it is used (Sefcik and Hockert, 2014; EUR-lex, 2020; ACCC, 2020).

While unit pricing offers consumers the opportunity to draw meaningful pricing comparisons between products, the UK government highlighted specific concerns relating to the inconsistent reporting of unit pricing, specifically in relation to, “the legibility of unit prices, inconsistencies for units of similar products and missing unit prices when there is a promotion” (Competition and Markets Authority, 2015, p. 10). These concerns not only cause unnecessary confusion for consumers when making comparisons between different products, irrespective of brand, size, and any ongoing promotional activity but highlight the need for consumers to possess the appropriate numerical proficiency when information is missing, and pricing inconsistencies arise during a grocery shop. Therefore, to determine the best grocery deals, consumers also need to obtain positive proficiency skills such as numeracy and problem solving. Numeracy and mental calculation skills can be viewed as important life skills for consumers to uphold, allowing consumers to compare prices regarding products and services (Sauble, 1955; Tan and Bogomolova, 2016). However, these skills can largely fluctuate amongst consumers in terms of age, gender and educational status (OECD, 2012). Within the UK, the most recent government research survey on adult skills (Skills for Life Survey: A Survey of Literacy, Numeracy and ICT Levels in England) found that between 2003 and 2011, numeracy skills displayed a decline and that it is most noticeable in the oldest generation. Further results revealed that numerical skills were lower amongst women (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2012). The OECD (2012) also reported a high proportion of adults scoring at or below Level 1 in numeracy across England and Northern Ireland (NI), meaning that they can only perform basic mathematical processes in common concrete contexts (OECD, 2012).

Research has found that higher levels of numerical skills predict a more thorough decision-making process in a retail setting, and thereby influence the quality of the purchase decision (Graffeo et al., 2015). On the other hand, having a low level of numerical skills can result in various negative outcomes in a retail setting. Difficulties locating prices on products and in accurately reading and computing numerical information can result in consumers reducing their product choices, making product choice decisions based on one attribute, in spending unusually long periods of time making product decisions, and in choosing the option that offers least value (Gau and Viswanathan, 2008). Lower levels of numerical skills and related difficulties in the retail store can also result in negative affective outcomes such as feelings of frustration and hopelessness and can lead to implementation of various coping strategies such as choosing stores with fewer choices and depending on others to help (Gau and Viswanathan, 2008).

Those with lower educational qualifications are likely to have lower numerical skills (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2012). This is problematic due to the link between lower education and lower income (Stryzhak, 2020), as complex promotional calculations which are misjudged by consumers could result in some spending more than they anticipated. This suggestion is justified by study findings which demonstrate that consumers will make more purchases and generate more revenue/profit for retailers when more complex promotional pricing strategies are used, such as sequential percentage discounts as opposed to single percentage discounts (Chen and Rao, 2007; Competition and Markets Authority, 2015; Amor, 2016). Difficulty calculating the best “deal” or best value option may be particularly problematic for those who are shopping within a particular specified budget for a household.

Zorbas et al. (2020) conducted a study on household purchasing patterns of price promoted and generic branded foods and found that a significantly greater proportion of purchases made by low- and middle-income households were price promoted and generic branded compared to high-income households, a pattern generally observed across food categories. Although Zorbas et al. (2020) did not use education as a mediating variable in their study, nor did they examine numerical skills, it could be considered that those in higher income households could have had higher education/numerical skills, and perhaps made calculated judgements regarding whether promotional items were a better offer than buying at the usual unit price. Tan and Bogomolova (2016) examined ability to comprehend numeric price promotions and found both age and income to be significant factors impacting on comprehension, indicating that young consumers with low-income were most likely to have difficulty accurately computing promotional offers. Although it has been indicated that numerical skills are differentiated by gender (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills (2012), Tan and Bogomolova (2016) found no significant difference in gender and ability to calculate price promotions. The link between gender and numerical skills/ability to calculate price promotions is of interest to further examine, considering that despite indication females have lower numerical skills than males (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2012), and despite maths and numerical skills often being viewed as a “males subject”, with 71% of men describing themselves as good/excellent at numeracy compared to 59% of women (Stanford, 2012), in many households females still carry the role of the main household shopper, with recent research finding that 77% of women (in a UK adult sample) carry out this task (Maynard, 2021). Therefore, it is of interest to compare how numerical skills and ability to work out the best deal on promotion is potentially differentiated by gender.

The overall aim of this study is to investigate the effect of consumer's numerical skills on their deal competency, and how both these variables relate to the demographic variables of education, age and gender. For the purposes of this study, “numerical skills” refers to the subjective appraisal of consumers quantitative ability, measured using an existing validated scale, the subjective numeracy scale (SNS) (Fagerlin et al., 2007). “Deal competency” for the purposes of this study refers to the ability of consumers to work out the best deal, measured using a novel measure which was informed by Hollywood et al. (2016). To the best of our knowledge, there has been no study conducted on consumers’ numerical literacy which has focused exclusively on the purchase of grocery items. There further exists a gap relating to how numerical skills and deal competency vary according to various demographic characteristics. Considering previous findings regarding how numerical skills can affect shopping behaviour, it is also of interest to examine if a significant relationship exists between numerical skills and deal competency and various consumer shopping behaviours such as amount and time spent in store, and behaviour relating to promotions. Specific research questions therefore are as follows:

RQ1.

What is the relationship, if any, between numerical skills, deal competency and the demographic characteristics of gender, age and education?

RQ2.

Is there a significant relationship between numerical skills and deal competency?

RQ3.

Is there a significant relationship between numerical skills, deal competency and consumer grocery shop buying behaviour (amount of money spent, amount of time spent and behaviour relating to promotions)?

Methods

An online survey was used to collect data from consumers. The survey was administered through the software program Qualtrics, and a convenience sample was used whereby the survey link was disseminated via email to undergraduate students within one university department and via social media (Twitter and Facebook) by members of the research team. This survey was further endorsed by the Consumer Council for NI who shared the survey web link to their consumer panel and across their social media channels. A total of 308 people completed the survey. The only screening criteria for this survey was that respondents should be 18 years or older. As the survey was disseminated online and as the scope of the study was interested in consumers generally rather than a specific respondent profile, there was no constraint on geographic location of respondents.

Survey design and measures

The survey consisted of four sections: demographic information, grocery shopping behaviour, numerical literacy evaluation (as measured by the SNS (Fagerlin et al., 2007)) and questions examining consumer ability to work out the best deal on promotion (deal competency) (as measured using a novel scale – see further elaboration below). Regarding demographics, this survey wanted to explore if there were consumer group differences (i.e. between genders, age groups and educational achievement) and numerical ability. The second section focused on self-reported grocery shopping behaviour to examine the level of the respondent's responsibility for grocery shopping in the household; how long they spend on a shop and their purchase intentions/attitudes towards promotions. Thirdly, the survey measured consumers numerical literacy using the SNS (Fagerlin et al., 2007), a validated eight-item scale constructed to subjectively measure consumers quantitative ability. The scale has eight-items: four relating to cognitive ability, for example “How good are you at working with fractions?”; and the other four items relate to preference for display of numeric information, for example “When reading the newspaper, how helpful do you find tables and graphs that are parts of a story?”. Each of the responses eight-item SNS scale questions was scored on a six-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating higher numerical ability (apart from one question which was reverse ranked). The reverse ranked question was reverse coded to create a new variable for use in analysis. All eight SNS items were computed to create a total SNS score ranging from a minimum of eight, to a maximum of 48, with the higher scores indicating higher numerical skills. The SNS was chosen for use in this study as it has been found to correlate well with objective measures of numerical skills, but is quicker and easier to administer, thereby reducing respondent burden and making it more likely they will finish completing the survey (Fagerlin et al., 2007). The fourth section examined consumer's deal competency regarding food and drink promotion using a novel exploratory scale measure constructed for the purposes of this study. This measure involved an experimental task that required respondents to assess (without using a calculator) which of the two options presented in each question was the better deal. The measure consisted of 16 questions, divided into four categories corresponding with the types of promotional offers most commonly used by grocery retailers, as identified by Hollywood et al. (2016). These included: price reductions, standalone offers, multi buys and mix and match deals. Each of the 16 questions relating to ability to discern the best promotional offer had two question responses: the correct answer (coded “1”) and the incorrect answer (coded as “0”). Participant answers were then totalled to calculate “deal competency”, with possible scores ranging from 0 to 16, with the higher scores indicating better deal competency. The authors recognise further testing for the reliability and validity of this scale is required using a representative sample. However, the development of this experimental scale is useful in providing some exploratory insight into people's ability to work out the best deal.

Data analysis

Data were analysed using SPSSv26. Following checks that the relevant assumptions were met, multiple regression analysis was used to examine the relationship between numerical skills and the demographic variables of gender, age and education (two education variables were included–highest educational achievement and highest mathematical achievement). A Poisson regression was used to assess the relationship between deal competency and the same demographic variables. Correlations (both Pearson and Spearman, as appropriate) were used to examine the relationship between numerical skills and ability to discern the best promotional offer on grocery products (deal competency), and other grocery shopping behaviours. Adjustments for multiple comparisons were made using Bonferroni corrections.

Ethics

Ethical approval was granted from the Ulster University Ethics Committee. No individuals under the age of 18 were obliged to complete this survey and this was clearly outlined in the survey guidelines. All information remained strictly confidential as no names or contact details were provided on the questionnaire.

Results

Respondent demographics

A total of 308 respondents (Table 1) completed the survey. The majority of the sample (82%) was from NI, 10.4% were from the Republic of Ireland, 6.5% were from regions in the UK other than NI, and a smaller number of participants were from Europe (n = 3) and Canada (n = 1). Over two-thirds of the sample (69%) was female, and regarding age, 57% were in the younger age groups, aged between 18 and 34. Almost half (47%) of respondents were in full time employment, while 30% were employed part time. There were a greater proportion of single individuals (59%) in the sample versus married (29%), and over half of respondents (57%) of respondents had no dependents living in their household. There was representation of respondents across social grades, with 56% of respondents categorised as ABC1 consumers (i.e. consumers from one of the three higher social and economic groups), therefore social/economic representation in this survey was fairly comparable to the UK as a whole which categorises 57% of consumers as ABC1 (Smith, 2019). Regarding mathematical achievement, 71% of participants achieved GCSE level maths or equivalent, and in terms of educational achievement, 40% of participants had attained a bachelor's degree while for 25% their highest educational achievement was secondary level.

Respondent buying behaviour

A total of 42% of respondents indicated that they were the principal household shopper, with a further 22% classifying this as a shared responsibility. Results showed that 67% of respondents spent £100 or less on their main weekly grocery shop, with approximately £65 being spent per week on the main grocery shop, and approximately £18 being spent per week on a top-up shop (Table 2). Regarding duration of the grocery shop, 46% of participants reported spending on average between 11 and 30 min carrying out the main grocery shop (Table 3), with 24% spending between 11 and 20 min on carrying out a top-up shop (Table 4). Most respondents (93%) stated they purchased promotional food items during a grocery shop. Overall, the mean SNS score for the total sample was 31.37 (SD = 8.27), and mean sample deal competency score was 13.49 (SD = 2.34) (Table 2).

Relationship between numerical skills and demographic variables

A multiple linear regression was conducted with predictor variables of gender, age, highest level of mathematical achievement and highest level of educational achievement (Table 5). The model was a significant fit (F(4) = 7.77, p < 0.001) and explained 9.4% of the variance in numerical skills as measured by the SNS (adjusted R2 = 0.082). Numerical skills were positively associated with being male (B = 2.67; t = 2.70; p = 0.007), and with highest level of mathematical achievement (B = 1.33; t = 3.19; p = 0.002). Age and highest educational achievement were not significant unique predictors of numerical skills.

Relationship between deal competency and demographic variables

A Poisson regression was conducted with predictor variables of gender, age, highest level of mathematical achievement and highest level of educational achievement and outcome variable of deal competency score. Goodness of fit statistics indicated that model fit was poor (deviance, value/df = 0.369). Omnibus results indicated that the model was not statistically significant (p = 0.084). Therefore, further interpretation of the model was not possible. Chi squared analyses indicated that there was no significant association between deal competency and gender, X2(10, N = 273) = 11.47, p = 0.322, or with age, X2(50, N = 273) = 50.38, p = 0.458. However, there were significant positive associations between highest level of education and deal competency score, X2(50, N = 273) = 72.31, p = 0.021 and between highest level of mathematical achievement and deal competency score, X2(50, N = 273) = 70.65, p = 0.029.

Numerical skills and deal competency

A Spearman's correlation analysis identified a moderate significant positive relationship between numerical skills and deal competency, rs(303) = 0.360, p < 0.001 (Table 6). A scatterplot (Figure 1) was used to further examine the relationship between both variables, indicating that higher numerical skills were associated with higher deal competency scores.

Numerical skills, deal competency and consumer buying behaviour

Correlation analyses (both Pearson and Spearman, used as appropriate) did not, on the main, identify significant correlations between numerical skills (SNS) and deal competency scores and average spend on grocery and top up shops (Table 6). Lower deal competency scores were significantly associated with increased average spend on promotional food items in top-up grocery shop, rs(208) = −0.195, p = 0.005 Pearson correlation analyses identified significant correlations (p < 0.001) between variables relating to average consumer spend on grocery and top up shops (Table 6).

Results of Spearman correlation analyses indicated no significant relationships (after correction for multiple comparisons) between numerical skills, deal competency and variables relating to time consumers spend on grocery and top up shops (Table 7). Regarding the relationship between times spent and money spent when shopping, analyses indicated that there were significant relationships between time spent on main and grocery shops, and average amount spent on these shops (Table 7). However, there was no significant relationship between time spent on main and grocery shops and average spend specifically on promotional food items (Table 7).

An independent samples t-test indicated that there was no significant difference between people who do and do not purchase foods on promotion both in terms of numerical skills (p = 0.072) and deal competency score (p = 0.148). Further correlation analysis (both Pearson and Spearman as appropriate) indicated no significant relationships between numerical skills and deal competency and variables relating to consumer buying behaviour and promotions after correcting for multiple comparisons (Table 8). Spearman correlation analyses established that the majority of variables relating to consumer buying behaviour and promotions were correlated with each other (Table 8).

Discussion

There was a moderate, significant correlation between numerical skills and deal competency. Being male and having a higher level of mathematical achievement were identified as significant predictors of numerical skills, while we could not identify significant predictors of deal competency. The finding that being male was a significant predictor of numerical skills appears to accord with previous findings that males have a greater level of numerical skills (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2012). Further study should therefore assess group (gender) differences to confirm (or disconfirm) this finding. Males' greater numerical skills did not however translate through to deal competency, as findings indicated that there is no significant difference in the accuracy of how males and females compute information relating to promotions. As the numerical skills measurement was subjective, and the deal competency measurement objective, it is possible that males may have overestimated their numerical capabilities on the subjective measure, thereby perhaps explaining why the finding that males had a greater level of numerical skills did not translate through to deal competency. Therefore, future studies should examine numerical skills using both subjective and objective measures. As females have been identified as often being the household primary shopper (Mintel, 2014; Maynard, 2021), it is possible that they have a greater aptitude for working out promotions, having more experience making similar calculations in a grocery environment, thus perhaps contributing towards explaining the finding that there was no significant difference between males and females deal competency scores, despite males being found to have greater numerical skills. Findings also suggest that older participants are more adept at working out promotions (indicated by deal competency score). Therefore perhaps it can also be hypothesised that older consumers have more experience with grocery shopping and are more accustomed to working out promotions than those in the youngest age category (18–24) who may live in a household where someone else is responsible for the grocery shopping (e.g. a parent), or if they are buying for themselves (i.e. a single household) they may be less likely to frequently be choosing bulk buying promotional items as they are buying a lower volume of goods.

Regarding education, the finding that mathematical achievement was related to numerical skills accorded with previous research (Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2012). The relationship between mathematical achievement and deal competency logically indicates that those with higher mathematical achievement will be better equipped to make accurate decisions about promotions. However, as a substantial number of the population have below average numerical skills (OECD, 2012), it is important that retailers are conscious to implement promotions which are as clear as possible for consumers, minimising risk of confusion, especially for those who may struggle to calculate the best deal (Competition and Markets Authority, 2015). Complex pricing strategies can be viewed negatively by consumers (Amor, 2016). Therefore, clarity around pricing and clear promotions can positively affect how consumers view a retailer and simplify their shopping decisions improving customer satisfaction and increasing repeat store visits.

Although there were no significant relationships between numerical skills and consumer buying behaviour, or between deal competency and consumer buying behaviour, it is acknowledged that one limitation of this experimental study is a relatively small sample size, and therefore in a larger sample significant effects may be observed. Although no significant relationships between deal competency and consumer buying behaviour were identified, as the scale is novel and has not been validated we cannot conclusively state that deal competency does not impact on consumer behaviour, and instead recommend further refining of the scale, i.e. more clearly defining and operationalising “deal competency” in an updated measure, and further testing to assess validity and reliability.

Regarding the implications of this study, awareness of how consumers differ in their numerical skills levels can incentivise retailers to be socially responsible and to empower consumers to make the best choice by providing clearer messaging around price promotion savings. This agenda however competes with retailers’ understandable corresponding goal of making profit. If consumers are misled by promotions or choose options offering less value this will be more profitable for the retailer. However, legislation on unit pricing and other recommendations by the Competition and Markets Authority (2015) following a complaint by consumer advocacy body “Which?” which encourages retailers to empower consumers by acting responsibly regarding pricing and promotional strategies. The study findings that consumers do differ in their level of numerical skills and in their ability to calculate the best deal contributes to the rationale for the use of mandatory unit pricing, however some limitations have been identified with unit pricing, such as legibility of unit prices, inconsistency of unit pricing, missing unit prices for promotional items and infrequent use of unit pricing by consumers (Competition and Markets Authority, 2015). Addressing these issues with unit pricing and raising consumer awareness of unit prices and their benefits can assist consumers in working out the best deal. The research also suggests some implications for consumer education, particularly amongst younger consumers. It may be advantageous to include teaching and evaluation of skills related to calculating promotions on the curriculum for all students, as budgeting and money skills are an important life skill. Students who study home economics and maths will learn these skills but those who do not pursue these subjects beyond basic required levels at school may be less equipped and have lower consumer efficacy in the future. Equipping consumers with the ability to make the best decisions as relate to value when purchasing food grocery items, can be considered to further accord with Cullen's (2015) conceptualisation of “food literacy” as the positive relationship built through social, cultural and environmental experiences with food enabling people to make decisions that support health, if ability to purchase the best deal on promotion facilitates making healthier food choices. Increasing consumer numerical skills and applying this knowledge to grocery shopping decision making therefore has potential to remove barriers to more healthful grocery shopping (Hollywood et al., 2013). Several studies have found price to be a primary influencing factor when grocery shopping (Glanz et al., 1998; Maillot et al., 2007; Waterlander et al., 2019), and that this attribute is particularly important for low-income consumers who may be less concerned about the healthiness of food (Jetter and Cassady, 2006; Drewnowski et al., 2007; Maillot et al., 2007). As price promotions have potential to influence population health through influencing consumption of certain food categories (Hollywood et al., 2016; Furey et al., 2019), consumer comprehension of promotions, and related influence of numerical skills is a research topic of interest not only from retail management, pricing strategy and consumer education perspectives, but also from a population health perspective. Further consideration is therefore recommended with regards to how best to educate consumers and provide them with the agency and skills to choose the best deal when grocery shopping, in accordance with their needs and budget, particularly for those consumers with lower levels of numerical skills (and literacy) who have been identified as being underrepresented in research, and with experiencing greater challenges and reliance on coping strategies when making purchase decisions in the retail environment (Gau and Viswanathan, 2008).

Conclusion

This study identified a positive relationship between numerical skills and deal competency, indicating that those with greater numerical skills will be more adept at working out the best deal in store. Those with lower numerical skills are therefore at a disadvantage in the retail environment with regards to making an informed decision. Although retailers have competing priorities with regards to maximising sales and increasing profits, it is nonetheless recommended that standardised unit pricing and clear promotional strategies should be used by retailers to empower consumers and build store trust; and thereby increasing store repeat visits and sales. Although this study did not find numerical skills and deal competency to have any significant relationship with consumer buying behaviour variables considered, findings regarding demographic differences with regards to numerical skills and deal competency accord with the above recommendation, and further rationalise the suggestion for increasing consumer education around making the most informed decision in store with regards to cost savings, for example related education in schools. Educating consumers and empowering them with the skills and knowledge to make informed choices in a grocery shopping environment is useful to aid consumers shopping on a budget, particularly low-income consumers, and has the potential to encourage more healthful grocery shopping, if price is a perceived barrier to making healthy choices, thereby consumer education with regards to numerical skills and deal competency can further contribute towards public health agendas for the population.

Figures

Scatterplot of relationship between deal competency score and numerical skills

Figure 1

Scatterplot of relationship between deal competency score and numerical skills

Respondent demographics

Percentage
Age
18–2444%
25–3423%
35–4414%
45–5415%
55–644%
Gender
Male31%
Female69%
Location
United Kingdom89%
Republic of Ireland10%
Occupation
Student34%
Unemployed2%
Homemaker/Retired/Carer6%
Part time employment (up to 29 h)12%
Full time employment (30+ hours per week)47%
Marital status
Single59.11%
Married/Cohabiting38%
Divorced/separated/widowed3%
No. of dependents in the household
058%
113%
215%
3+15%
Highest level of mathematical achievement
Key stage 3 maths3%
Maths GCSE (or equivalent)71%
Maths AS level2%
Maths A level16%
University degree7%
Career related to mathematics2%
Highest level of educational achievement
Primary school1%
Secondary school25%
Apprenticeship/NVQ level 2 or 311%
Bachelor degree40%
Postgraduate degree (e.g. PGCE, Masters or PhD)19%
Social grade*
A–high managerial, administrative or professional8%
B–Intermediate managerial, administrative or professional34%
C1 - Supervisory, clerical and junior managerial, administrative or professional14%
C2 - Skilled manual workers13%
D–Semi and unskilled manual workers27%
E–state pensioners, casual or lowest grade workers and unemployed with state benefits only5%

Note(s): *Occupations cited by respondents were categorised according to the six main social grades (NRC, 2021)

Summary descriptive statistics

Mean (SD)MinMax
Subjective numeracy scale score31.37 (8.27)848
Deal competency score13.49 (2.34)616
Average spend on main grocery shop per week (£)64.93 (40.10)0250
Average spend on promotional food items in main grocery shop per week (£)19.22 (20.19)0203
Average spend on top-up shop per week (£)17.72 (14.17)0100
Average spend on promotional food items in top-up grocery shop per week (£)7.78 (20.04)0202

Frequency of responses to “On average, how long did you spend on your last grocery shop?”

N (%)
5–10 min37 (12.0)
11–30 min142 (46.1)
31–60 min114 (37.0)
More than 60 min15 (4.9)

Frequency of responses to “On average, how long did you spend on your last top-up grocery shop?”

N (%)
Less than 5 min83 (26.9)
6–10 min133 (43.2)
11–20 min75 (24.4)
More than 20 min17 (5.5)

Linear regression predicting numerical skills

Adjusted R20.082**
PredictorsUnstandardised Bt
Age1.370.17
Gender0.149*2.70
Highest level of mathematical attainment3.19*0.002
Highest level of educational attainment2.460.014

Note(s): ** < 0.001; * < 0.05

Correlations between numerical skills, deal competency and average spend per week

Deal competency scoreAverage spend on main grocery shop per weekAverage spend on promotional food items in main grocery shop per weekAverage spend on top-up shop per weekAverage spend on promotional food items in top-up grocery shop per week
SNS scorer0.360a−0.011−0.0680.0440.014
p<0.001*0.8680.2760.5270.841
N271249256211210
Deal competency scoreRho 0.050a0.007a0.022a−0.195a
p0.4290.9120.7490.005
N250257211210
Average grocery shop spend per weekr 0.5450.5260.268
p<0.001*<0.001*<0.001*
N247205204
Average spend on promotional food items in main grocery shop per weekr 0.3990.526
p<0.001*<0.001*
N207208
Average spend on top-up shop per weekr 0.581
p<0.001*
N205

Note(s): aSpearman correlations, all other correlations are Pearson; * Remains significant after correction for multiple comparisons

Spearman correlations between numerical skills, deal competency score and time spent shopping

Time spent on main grocery shopTime spent on top-up grocery shop
SNS scoreRho0.0330.031
p0.5710.585
N305305
Deal competency scoreRho0.1200.023
p0.0470.699
N273273
Average grocery shop spend per weekRho0.2770.059
p<0.001*0.349
N252252
Average spend on promotional food items in main grocery shop per weekRho0.054−0.130
p0.3830.037
N259259
Average spend on top-up shop per weekRho0.1730.213
p0.0110.002*
N213213
Average spend on promotional food items in top-up grocery shop per weekRho−0.038−0.018
p0.5790.793
N212212
Time spent on main grocery shopRho0.370
p<0.001*
N308

Note(s): *Remains significant after correction for multiple comparisons

Correlations between numerical skills, deal competency and shopping behaviour

Deal competency scoreI shop around for special offersI check newspapers and fliers for the best offersI shop at a number of supermarkets for their best offersNumber of items in main grocery shop on promotion per weekNumber of items in top-up grocery shop on promotion per week
SNS scoreRho0.360−0.011−0.052−0.009−0.047a−0.014a
p<0.001*0.8450.3660.8720.4490.829
N271305305305259255
Deal competency scoreRho−0.101−0.0950.003−0.039−0.123
p0.0970.1180.9600.5320.050
N273273273260256
I shop around for special offersRho0.3260.6440.2430.178
p<0.001*<0.001*<0.001*0.004
N308308262258
I check newspapers and fliers for the best offersRho0.4220.2080.135
p<0.0010.0010.030
N308262258
I shop at a number of supermarkets for their best offersRho0.2070.212
p0.001*0.001*
N262258
Number of items in main grocery shop on promotion per weekr 0.760a
p<0.001*
N253

Note(s): aPearson correlations, all other correlations are Spearman; *Remains significant after correction for multiple comparisons

References

Australian Competition and Consumer Commission (ACCC) (2020), “Unit pricing code”, available at: https://www.accc.gov.au/business/industry-codes/unit-pricing-code (accessed 5 August 2021).

Amor, I.B. (2016), “Effects of complex price communication on fairness: case of a sequential communication”, Marketing Studies, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 38-45.

Babin, B.J., Chebat, J.C. and Michon, R. (2004), “Perceived appropriateness and its effect on quality, affect and behaviour”, Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 11, pp. 287-298.

Bogomolova, S., Dunn, S., Taylor, J. and Volpe, R.J. (2015), “Price promotion landscape in the US and UK: depicting retail practice to inform future research agenda”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 25, pp. 1-11.

Bogomolova, S., Oppewal, H., Cohen, J. and Yao, J. (2020), “How the layout of a unit price label affects eye-movements and product choice: an eye-tracking investigation”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 111, pp. 102-116.

Carlson, J.R. and Kukar-Kinney, M. (2020), “Investigating discounting of discounts in an online context: the mediating effect of discount credibility and moderating effect of online daily deal promotions”, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Vol. 41, pp. 153-160.

Caviola, S., Carey, E., Mammarella, I.C. and Szucs, D. (2017), “Stress, time pressure, strategy selection and math anxiety in mathematics: a review of the literature”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 8, p. 1488.

Chen, H.A. and Rao, A. (2007), “When two plus two is not equal to four: errors in processing multiple percentage changes”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 34 No. 3, pp. 327-340.

Choi, P. and Coulter, K.S. (2012), “It's not all relative: the effects of mental and physical positioning of comparative prices on absolute versus relative discount assessment”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 88 No. 4, pp. 512-527.

Choi, C. and Mattila, A.S. (2014), “The effects of promotion framing on consumers' price perceptions: the moderating role of a personal sense of power”, Journal of Service Management, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 149-160.

CMA (2015), “Unit pricing: information for consumers”, available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/unit-pricing-information-for-consumers/unit-pricing-information-for-consumers (accessed 2 February 2021).

Competition and Markets Authority (2015), “Pricing practices in the groceries market”, available at: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/55a6c83540f0b61562000005/Groceries_Pricing_Super-Complaint_response.pdf (accessed 5 August 2021).

Cullen, T. (2015), “Food literacy: definition and framework for action”, Canadian Journal of Dietetic Practice and Research, Vol. 76 No. 3, pp. 140-145.

DelVecchio, D., Krishnan, S. and Smith, D. (2007), “Cents or percent? The effects of promotion framing on price expectations and choice”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 71 No. 3, pp. 158-170.

Department for Business, Innovation and Skills (2012), “The 2011 skills for life survey: a survey of literacy, numeracy and ICT levels in England”, available at: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/36000/12-p168-2011-skills-for-life-survey.pdf (accessed 6 August 2021).

Drewnowski, A., Monsivais, P., Maillot, M. and Darmon, N. (2007), “Low-energy density diets are associated with higher diet quality and higher diet costs in French adults”, Journal of the American Dietetic Association, Vol. 107 No. 6, pp. 1028-1032.

EUR-lex (2020), “Price indications on consumer products”, available at: https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?uri=LEGISSUM%3Al32032 (accessed 5 August 2021).

Fagerlin, A., Zikmund-Fisher, B.J., Ubel, P.A., Jankovic, A., Derry, H.A. and Smith, D.M. (2007), “Measuring numeracy without a math test: development of the subjective numeracy scale”, Medical Decision Making, Vol. 27, pp. 672-680.

Furey, S., McLaughlin, C., Hollywood, L.E., Burns, A., McMahon-Beattie, U., Price, R.K., Humphreys, P., McCarthy, M., Collins, A., Raats, M., Tatlow-Golden, M., Dean, M. and Murrin, C. (2019), What's on Offer? The Types of Food and Drink on Price Promotion in Retail Outlets in the Republic of Ireland, SafeFood, Cork, available at: https://www.safefood.eu/SafeFood/media/SafeFoodLibrary/Documents/Publications/Research%20Reports/Whats-on-offer.pdf.

Garaus, M., Wagner, U. and Kummer, C. (2015), “Cognitive fit, retail shopper confusion and shopping value: empirical investigation”, Business Research, Vol. 68, pp. 1003-1011.

Gau, R. and Viswanathan, M. (2008), “The retail shopping experience for low-literate consumers”, Journal of Research for Consumers, Vol. 15.

Glanz, K., Basil, M., Maibachm, E., Goldberg, J. and Snyder, D. (1998), “Why Americans eat what they do: taste, nutrition, cost, convenience, and weight control concerns as influences on food consumption”, Journal of the American Dietetic Association, Vol. 98 No. 10, pp. 1118-1126.

Gordon-Hecker, T., Pittarello, A., Shalvi, S. and Roskes, M. (2020), “Buy-one-get-one-free deals attract more attention than percentage deals”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 111, pp. 128-134.

Graffeo, M., Polonio, L. and Bonini, N. (2015), “Individual differences in competent consumer choice: the role of cognitive reflection and numeracy skills”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 6 No. 844.

Harrison-Walker, L.J. (1995), “The import of illiteracy to marketing communication”, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 50-62.

Hollywood, L.E., Cuskelly, G.J., O'Brien, M., McConnon, A., Barnett, J., Raats, M.M. and Dean, M. (2013), “Healthful grocery shopping. Perceptions and barriers”, Appetite, Vol. 70, pp. 119-126.

Hollywood, L., Furey, S., Burns, A., McMahon-Beattie, U., Price, R., Duffy, M., Dowler, E., Livingstone, B., Humphreys, P., Moore, C. and McCullagh, F. (2016), “A three-stage investigation into the balance of healthy versus less healthy food promotions among Northern Ireland food retailers”, available at: https://www.food.gov.uk/sites/default/files/media/document/fs305021fsasummaryreportmarch16.pdf (accessed 11 March 2021).

Jae, H. and Delvecchio, D. (2004), “Decision making by low-literacy consumers in the presence of point-of-purchase information”, Journal of Consumer Affairs, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 342-354.

Jetter, K.M. and Cassady, D.L. (2006), “The availability and cost of healthier food alternatives”, American Journal of Preventative Medicine, Vol. 30 No. 1, pp. 38-44.

Lee, L., Lee, M.P., Bertini, M., Zauberman, M.B. and Ariely, D. (2015), “Money, time and the stability of consumer preferences”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 52 No. 2, pp. 184-199.

Maillot, M., Darmon, N., Vieux, F. and Drewnowski, A. (2007), “Low energy density and high nutritional quality are each associated with higher diet costs in French adults”, American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, Vol. 86 No. 3, pp. 690-696.

Maynard, M. (2021), “Shopping habits: how do they differ between men and women?”, available at: https://www.thegrocer.co.uk/consumer-trends/shopping-habits-how-do-they-differ-between-men-and-women/656959.article (accessed 9 August 2021).

Mintel (2014), “Supermarkets: more than just food retailing”, available at: https://store.mintel.com/report/supermarkets-more-than-just-food-retailing-uk-november-2013?utm_source=reports.mintel.com&utm_medium=redirect&utm_campaign=301redir (accessed 13 October 2021).

National Readership Survey (NRC) (2021), “Social grade”, available at: http://www.nrs.co.uk/nrs-print/lifestyle-and-classification-data/social-grade/ (accessed 9 August 2021).

OECD (2012), “Survey of adult skills first results (UK): England and Northern Ireland”, available at: https://www.oecd.org/skills/piaac/Country%20note%20-%20United%20Kingdom.pdf (accessed 6 August 2021).

Peters, E. (2012), “Beyond comprehension: the role of numeracy in judgments and decisions”, Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 31-35.

Sauble, I. (1955), “Development of ability to estimate and to compute mentally”, The Arithmetic Teacher, Vol. 2, pp. 33-39.

Sefcik, D. and Hockert, C. (2014), “Unit pricing guide ‘A best practice approach to unit pricing’”, available at: https://www.govinfo.gov/content/pkg/GOVPUB-C13-4851a46324cc86af742242cfc5a93d9a/pdf/GOVPUB-C13-4851a46324cc86af742242cfc5a93d9a.pdf (accessed 5 August 2021).

Sinayev, A. and Peters, E. (2015), “Cognitive reflection vs calculation in decision making”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 6 No. 532.

Smith, M. (2019), “How well do ABC1 and C2DE correspond with our own class identity?”, available at: https://yougov.co.uk/topics/politics/articles-reports/2019/11/25/how-well-do-abc1-and-c2de-correspond-our-own-class (accessed 6 August 2021).

Stanford, P. (2012), “Make Britain Count: are girls really worse at maths than boys?”, The Telegraph, available at: http://www.telegraph.co.uk/education/maths-reform/9126371/Make-Britain-Count-Are-girls-really-worse-at-maths-than-boys.html (accessed 6th February 2012).

Stryzhak, O. (2020), “The relationship between education, income, economic freedom and happiness”, HS Web of Conferences, Vol. 75, 03004.

Tan, P.J. and Bogomolova, S. (2016), “A descriptive analysis of consumer's price promotion literacy skills”, International Journal of Retail and Distribution Management, Vol. 44 No. 12, pp. 1223-1244.

Waterlander, W.E., Jiang, Y., Nghiem, N., Eyles, H., Wilson, N., Cleghorn, C., Genç, M., Swinburn, B., Ni Mhurchu, C. and Blakely, T. (2019), “The effect of food price changes on consumer purchases: a randomised experiment”, The Lancet, Vol. 4 No. 8, pp. E394-E405.

Weller, J.A., Dieckmann, N.F., Tusler, M., Mertz, C.K., Burns, W.J. and Peters, E. (2013), “Development and testing of an abbreviated numeracy scale: a Rasch analysis approach”, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 26, pp. 198-212.

Yao, J. and Oppewal, H. (2016), “Unit pricing matters more when consumers are under time pressure”, European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 50 Nos 5/6, pp. 1094-1114.

Zorbas, C., Eyles, H., Orellana, L., Peeters, A., Ni Mhurchu, C., Riesenberg, D. and Backholer, K. (2020), “Do purchases of price promoted and generic branded foods and beverages vary according to food category and income level? Evidence from a consumer research panel”, Appetite, Vol. 144 No. 104481.

Further reading

Adkins, N. and Ozanne, J. (2005), “The low literate consumer”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 32 No. 1, pp. 93-105.

Consumer Council Northern Ireland (CCNI) (2011), “Hard to Stomach: the impact of rising food costs for Northern Ireland consumers”, available at: http://www.consumercouncil.org.uk/filestore/documents/Hard_to_Stomach1.pdf (accessed 2 February 2021).

Collins, A., Kavanagh, E., Cronin, J. and George, R. (2014), “Money, mavens, time, and price search: modelling the joint creation of utilitarian and hedonic value in grocery shopping”, Marketing Management, Vol. 30 Nos 7-8, pp. 719-746.

Fernandes, D., Lynch, J.G. Jr and Netemeyer, R.G. (2014), “Financial literacy, financial education, and downstream financial behaviors”, Management Science, Vol. 60 No. 8, pp. 1861-1883.

Jae, H., DelVecchio, D.S. and Childers, T.L. (2010), “Are low-literate and high-literate consumers different? Applying resource-matching theory to ad processing across literacy levels”, Journal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 21 No. 3, pp. 312-323.

Krishna, A., Briesch, R., Lehmann, D.R. and Yuan, H. (2002), “A meta-analysis of the impact of price presentation on perceived savings”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 78 No. 2, pp. 101-118.

Laroche, M., Pons, F., Zgolli, N., Cervellon, M.C. and Kim, C. (2003), “A model of consumer response to two retail sales promotion techniques”, Business Research, Vol. 56, pp. 513-522.

Mitchell, V.W. and Papavassiliou, V. (1999), “Marketing causes and implications of consumer confusion”, Journal of Product and Brand Management, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 319-342.

Reutskaja, E., Nagel, R., Camerer, C.F. and Rangel, A. (2011), “Search dynamics in consumer choice under time pressure: an eye-tracking study”, The American Economic Review, Vol. 101 No. 2, pp. 900-926.

Saini, R., Rao, R.S. and Monga, A. (2010), “Is that deal worth my time? The interactive effect of relative and referent thinking on willingness to seek a bargain”, Marketing, Vol. 74 No. 1, pp. 34-48.

Sinha, I. and Smith, M.F. (2000), “Consumers' perceptions of promotional framing of price”, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 257-275.

Yadav, M.S. and Monroe, K.B. (1993), “How buyers perceive savings in a bundle price: an examination of a bundle's transaction value”, Marketing Research, Vol. 30 No. 3, pp. 350-358.

Corresponding author

Emma Beacom can be contacted at: emma.beacom@ucc.ie

Related articles