Emotional intelligence and fraud tendency: a survey of future accountants in Nigeria

Collins Sankay Oboh (Department of Accounting, University of Lagos, Lagos, Nigeria)

European Journal of Management Studies

ISSN: 2183-4172

Article publication date: 11 April 2023

Issue publication date: 10 May 2023

2775

Abstract

Purpose

This study examines the association between emotional intelligence (EI) and the tendency of future accountants to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud.

Design/methodology/approach

The study adopts a survey methodology and uses a questionnaire containing a fraud scenario and EI construct to gather data from 225 participants. It performed a Cronbach alpha to assess the measurement parameters consistency of EI and fraud tendency and employed Pearson correlation and regression analysis to test its hypothesis.

Findings

The study found that future accountants in Nigeria are emotionally intelligent and have a high fraud tendency. Also, it found a significant and positive association between EI and fraud tendency, suggesting that future accountants that are emotionally intelligent have a higher tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud. In addition, the study found that academic intelligence, a control variable, positively associates with fraud tendency.

Practical implications

The study offers rare insights into the fraud tendency of future accountants, which would benefit the counter fraud community in Nigeria and other developing countries. Recruiters and employers will find the study beneficial in decision-making on job recruitment, placements and moral orientation for prospective accountant employees.

Originality/value

The study is the first to directly associate EI with the fraud tendency of future accountants from a developing country with high fraud profile and underdeveloped counter fraud strategy. Thus, it provides a benchmark for future studies in other developing countries.

Keywords

Citation

Oboh, C.S. (2023), "Emotional intelligence and fraud tendency: a survey of future accountants in Nigeria", European Journal of Management Studies , Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 3-22. https://doi.org/10.1108/EJMS-05-2022-0038

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023, Collins Sankay Oboh

License

Published in European Journal of Management Studies. Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence maybe seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Fraud is unpleasant and causes great harm, especially occupational fraud. It impacts the quality of life of individuals, organizations and the public (Association of Certified Fraud Examiners [ACFE], 2020; Albrecht et al., 2019). At its highest level, occupational fraud consists of three primary categories: asset misappropriation, financial statement fraud and corruption (ACFE, 2020). It remains a leading threat to the growth and survival of businesses, and it permeates into public and private organizations, causing economic uneasiness in developed and developing countries, as over $3.3 bn is lost to occupational fraud annually (ACFE, 2020; Central Intelligence Agency [CIA], 2017; Nanda et al., 2019). A survey of 125 countries by the ACFE in 2020 confirms the rise and threat posed by occupational fraud to the global economy and security (ACFE, 2020), and amid these rising fraud incidences, accountants are accused of being enablers of occupational fraud (Adekoya et al., 2020; Matthews and Gandel, 2015; Oboh et al., 2020; Oboh and Omolehinwa, 2022; Rezaee and Riley, 2010; Sikka and Lehman, 2015). Critical accounting scholars (e.g. Bakre, 2007; Griffin, 2015; Oboh, 2019; Sikka, 2009; Sikka, 2017; Uche, 2002) allege that accountants use their expertise to aid the management and directors of companies to deliberately alter their financial statements to evade taxes, siphon money and manipulate share prices. The Enron scandal is a classic case that depicts accountants as accomplices of occupational fraud with management and directors of companies, especially financial statement fraud, to meet market expectations and impress investors (Ajekwe and Ibiamke, 2017; Albrecht et al., 2019; Nanda et al., 2019; Rezaee, 2005; Rezaee and Riley, 2010). Recent fraud cases of Volkswagen, Toshiba, Nissan and other multinational companies involving accountants are shreds of evidence necessitating research response and counter fraud interventions (Gaspar et al., 2021; Oboh, 2019; Oboh and Omolehinwa, 2022).

In response to the problem of fraud, practitioners and academics have created many explanations as to what conditions stir up the tendency and enable individuals to commit fraud, some of which have had a considerable influence on the counter fraud community (Marks, 2019; Vousinas, 2019; Wells, 2017). The most influential explanation is the fraud triangle, developed from the work of Cressey (1953). The fraud triangle suggests that an act of occupational fraud occurs when an individual under pressure, caused by a non-shareable financial problem, has the opportunity to engage in fraud and manages to justify committing an act of fraud to themselves – a process labeled rationalization (Cressey, 1953; Wells, 2017), and this fraud model has been considerably influential on the counter fraud community, which subsequently has focused almost all of its counter fraud recommendations upon reducing opportunity, and to a much smaller extent pressure, rather than rationalization. Rationalization in the fraud triangle is the catalyst, internal to individuals, which determines the occurrence of fraud despite external pressure and opportunity. It aligns with the behavioral tendencies of individuals, and we learned from Ajzen's (1991) theory of planned behavior (TPB) and Ajzen and Fishbein's (2005) theory of reasoned action (TRA) that the actions of an individual are guided, and his behavior is predicted based on his behavioral tendency or intention. Ajzen (1991) suggests that an individual's attitude, which is a makeup of his/her emotions, values and beliefs, is an intrinsic factor that leads to the formation of a tendency or an intention towards a behavior. Previous literature on behavioral psychology and moral development signals that fraud tendency is an internal behavioral proclivity that interrelates with the emotions, ethics, values, moral development and beliefs of individuals (Ajzen and Fishbein, 2005; Awang et al., 2019; Gaspar et al., 2021; Ismail, 2015; Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Kohlberg, 1973; Rest, 1979; Jones, 1991). Thus, reflecting on Ajzen's (1991) TPB and inferences from Ajzen and Fishbein (2005) and previous literature, it is expected that the tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud may be contingent on an individual's emotional intelligence (EI), a possibility this study seeks to explore.

In time past, renowned scholars (e.g. Angelidis and Ibrahim, 2011; Dulewicz and Higgs, 2005; Gransberry, 2022; Gómez-Leal et al., 2022; Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Law et al., 2008; Lone and Lone, 2018; Schlaegel et al., 2022) explored a diverse range of EI-related issues (e.g. effective leadership, moral judgment, job performance and satisfaction and ethical decision-making [EDM], among others). But an association between EI and the tendency of future accountants to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud appears not to have been considered in previous studies. And whether EI plays a role in predicting occupational fraud in organizations is a question the counter fraud community needs to consider to update their counter fraud interventions and recommendations. Though empirical research associating EI with fraud tendency is limited and prior results vary (Gaspar et al., 2021; Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Keefer et al., 2018; Rahmatullah et al., 2018; Yang et al., 2018), Ismail (2015) suggests EI associates with the cognitive processes of individuals in different decision-making situations, and as an intrinsic behavioral disposition, EI signposts the meta-level ability of individuals to handle emotions and use them in ways that lead to positive outcomes (Goleman, 1998; Ismail, 2015; van Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004). Therefore, it is expected that EI may associate with fraud tendency (Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Sulastri and Kasanah, 2021), presupposing that a high EI will lead to a low fraud tendency. Thus, hypothesizing an association between EI and fraud tendency will contribute new information to existing fraud literature, which would interest the counter fraud community and fraud investigators.

Reflecting on the limitations of previous research and the call for counter fraud interventions, this study examines the association between EI and fraud tendency, aiming to add new information to existing literature and make up for the limited research on fraud tendency. Thus, the study makes three contributions to the current literature. First, the study adds to the literature on the direct association between EI and the tendency of future accountants to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud, which appears to be among the first to do so. Therefore, fraud theorists and the counter fraud community would find the study beneficial in recommending counter fraud interventions that may curb occupational fraud. Second, focusing on future accountants in Nigeria as a case study adds some uniqueness to empirical evidence regarding the developing world. Whereas most studies on occupational fraud are from developed countries and are limited to external factors focusing on managers, directors and other employees of companies, the current study explores a direct relationship between EI and the fraud tendency of future accountants from a developing country, making its finding a significant contribution to the literature. Besides, Nigeria represents developing countries with high corruption profiles (Bakre, 2007; Oboh, 2019; The Central Bank of Nigeria (CBN), 2017; The Nigeria Inter-Bank Settlement System (NIBSS), 2020; Transparency International, 2020). Therefore, the evidence in this study would serve as a benchmark for future research in other developing countries with similar fraud profiles. In addition, the study offers rare insights into the fraud tendency of future accountants, which recruiters, employers and human resource managers would find useful in making decisions relating to job recruitment, placements and moral orientation for prospective accountant employees. Lastly, this study combines two stages (moral judgment and moral intention) of Rest's (1979) EDM model and applies Cressey's (1953) fraud triangle to create a scenario and scale to measure fraud tendency. Future researchers can adopt the scenario and scale to measure EI and fraud tendency as the scenario reflects a real-life fraud situation mostly found in developing countries with high-fraud profiles.

The other parts of the study include the literature review, theoretical framework and hypothesis development on EI and the fraud tendency. The methodology adopted is then described, followed by a presentation and discussion of the results. Lastly, the conclusions, implications, limitations and suggestions for future research are discussed.

2. Literature review and hypothesis development

Fraud is any act proscribed by law as fraudulent, including but not limited to cheating, false representations and abuse of power (The United Kingdom Fraud Act, 2006; The Nigerian Criminal Code Act, 2004; United Nations Development Programme, 2017). It is a range of crimes involving dishonest acts and means that human ingenuity invents, which is resorted to by one individual, to get an advantage over another (Albrecht et al., 2019; Doig, 2012; Rufus et al., 2015). Among the different categories of fraud (e.g. person-to-person fraud, Internet and identity fraud), occupational fraud is one of the costliest forms of financial crime (ACFE, 2020). It is fraud against an organization that can be carried out by any individual who is a member of the organization, whether they are ordinary employees or directors (Ratmono and Frendy, 2022). It has three mutually exclusive categories: asset misappropriation, financial statement fraud and corruption (ACFE, 2020; Holtfreter, 2005; Nanda et al., 2019). And following the Enron and Andersen scandal in 2002 and subsequent cases of occupational fraud, accountants have come under stern criticisms of being enablers of occupational fraud, accused of using their expertise and intelligence to aid the management and directors of companies to alter their financial statements (Bakre, 2007; Griffin, 2015; Oboh and Omolehinwa, 2022; Rezaee and Riley, 2010; Sikka, 2017). Responding to the problem of fraud, academics and experts rely on models to explain the reason individuals rationalize and engage in occupational fraud; of which, four models dominate the literature and three (fraud scale, fraud diamond and fraud pentagon) of which are variants of the oldest and most referenced model – the fraud triangle (Wells, 2017; Vousinas, 2019; Dorminey et al., 2012). The triangle model, created from Cressey's (1953) research, suggests an act of fraud occurs when an individual under pressure caused by a non-shareable financial problem has the opportunity to engage in fraud and rationalizes committing an act of fraud to themselves (Albrecht et al., 2019; Andon et al., 2015; Awang and Ismail, 2018; Cressey, 1953; Wells, 2017). In principle, pressure and opportunity are external stimuli of fraud, while rationalization is the internal catalyst that makes fraud happen. Thus, it is all important for the counter fraud community to look beyond external factors like pressure, opportunity and ethical climates and consider intrinsic influences such as values, beliefs, emotions and ideologies that may predict the fraud tendency or rationalization behaviors of individuals.

In linking Cressey's (1953) fraud triangle to Ajzen's (1991) TPB, this study considers pressure and opportunity as the external forces that excite individuals to form an intention to commit occupational fraud, which rationalization causes to materialize (Albrecht et al., 2019; Baucus, 1994; Cressey, 1953; Wells, 2017), and relying on the arguments of prior moral and behavioral studies, it is logical to note that the fraud tendency of an individual is the crossing point of his/her moral judgment and moral intention that leads him/her to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud (Armstrong et al., 2003; Cressey, 1953; Marks, 2019; Rest, 1979; Schwartz, 2016; Wood et al., 2021). By implication, fraud tendency is more or less contingent on the extent to which an individual rationalizes and excuses fraud by integrating dubious cognitions into his moral judgment and intention (see Ajzen, 1991; Dewi and Anisykurlillah, 2021; Forsyth, 1980; Kohlberg, 1973; Marks, 2012), which explains the reason not everyone exposed to the same pressure and opportunity ends up committing fraud. Thus, fraud tendency is the behavioral proclivity in individuals that makes them rationalize fraud allures, leading to fraudulent behavior, which previous research suggests may be contingent on the attitudes, beliefs, values, ideologies and emotions of individuals (Ajzen, 2012; Ajzen and Fishbein, 2005; Ajzen and Madden, 1986; Forsyth, 1980; Gaspar et al., 2021; Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Kohlberg, 1973; Rahmatullah et al., 2018; Rest, 1979).

Considering the possible tie between an individual's behavioral tendencies and emotions, beliefs and values suggested by Ajzen (1991) and Ajzen and Fishbein (2005), the current study explores whether EI associates with fraud tendency, which may be a plausible behavioral intervention strategy to help the counter fraud community reduce fraud rationalization. Though empirical studies on EI and fraud tendency are scarce (Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Keefer et al., 2018; Yang et al., 2018), previous research on EI and cognitive processes (Ismail, 2015), EI and moral judgment (Ismail and Rasheed, 2019; Sulastri and Kasanah, 2021) and EI and deception (Gaspar et al., 2021) provides considerable evidence to expect an association between EI and fraud tendency, and while the concept of EI is not new, Ismail (2015) notes that EI is a behavioral temperament that signposts the meta-level ability of individuals to handle emotions and use them in decision-making. It has five components, namely self-awareness (being in touch with one's feelings and using the knowledge to make guided decisions), self-regulation (the ability to redirect disruptive moods to achieve goals), motivation (the drive to achieve impactful results), empathy (understanding and caring for the emotions of others) and social skill (building teams and managing relationships), all put together guide the thinking, actions and behaviors of individuals (Goleman, 1998; Ismail, 2015; Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2010; van Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004). Evidence from renowned scholars and psychologists also suggests that EI is associated with diverse behavioral tendencies and dispositions. For example, Mandell and Pherwani (2003) and Bradbury and Graves (2005) reported an association between EI and effective leadership. Angelidis and Ibrahim (2011), Deshpande and Joseph (2009) and Mesmer-Magnus et al. (2010) found that EI impacts ethical decision-making, while others reported an influence of EI on job performance (Dulewicz and Higgs, 2005; Law et al., 2008) and job commitment and satisfaction (Sy et al., 2006; Wong and Law, 2002). But despite the extensive research on EI in the business domain, very little research has been undertaken to associate EI with individual morality, and none on its association with the tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud (Gaspar et al., 2021; Ismail, 2015).

The few available studies on EI and morality suggest that EI goes beyond individual morality and could predict the tendency of individuals to rationalize and engage in occupation fraud. For example, Ismail (2015), Ismail and Rasheed (2019) and Sulastri and Kasanah (2021) examined and found an association between EI and moral judgment. They suggest that the ability to manage the emotions of self and others is critical to making moral judgments, which is closely linked to moral intention and moral behavior in Rest's (1979) EDM model, suggesting a possibility of EI influencing an individual's decision whether or not to engage in occupational fraud. In associating emotional quotient (EQ) with the tendency of local government officers in Indonesia to commit fraud, Rahmatullah et al. (2018) suggest EQ or EI influences fraud tendency negatively, indicating that fraud tendency is lower in individuals with high EQ. But recent research by Gaspar et al. (2021) provides a theoretical model and propositions on the association between EI and deception (characterized as fraud tendency), and in sharp contrast to Rahmatullah et al. (2018), Gaspar et al. (2021) conclude that emotionally intelligent individuals are the most likely to deceive, presupposing that individuals with high EI have a higher tendency to rationalize, commit and conceal a fraud.

In theory, three possible outcomes on the association between EI and fraud tendency may be observed, which are (1) a positive; (2) a negative and (3) a no association. But reflecting on Rahmatullah et al. (2018) and Gaspar et al. (2021), there are reasons to agree with both studies that EI could either lower or heighten fraud tendency in individuals. Going with the argument of Rahmatullah et al. (2018), with a high level of self-regulation, empathy and social skills, emotionally intelligent individuals are compassionate and considerate of others. They can control the tendency to commit fraud, considering the aftermath of the victims. In accordance with Goleman (1998), emotionally intelligent individuals can understand and regulate their emotions irrespective of any situational pressures and may likely abstain from fraud as an alternative to solving their financial problems. But considering Gaspar et al.’s (2021) argument, it would require EI to motivate and override one's emotions (or guilty feelings) to rationalize, engage in and conceal fraud. Besides, Wolfe and Hermanson (2004) and Marks (2019) argued individually that fraud perpetrators possess the capability and competence to diagnose, navigate and exploit fraud opportunities, and one such capability is the ability to be sturdy emotionally as to rationalize and override any guilty feelings of committing fraud. Referencing Marks' (2019) fraud pentagon model, a negative association is expected between EI and fraud tendency because, with the inclusion of arrogance in the pentagon model, Marks (2019) equates arrogance with hard conscience and greed, suggesting fraud perpetrators are possibly lacking in EI which makes them disregard the feelings of their victims. Thus, the current study assumes that individuals who are low in EI are egoistic and self-centered, having little or no empathy for the victims of fraud, not minding whether it may lead to the loss of a victim's life. But emotionally intelligent individuals may consider the emotions of others and put their interests first, desisting from fraud than hurting the feelings of others (Dewi and Anisykurlillah, 2021; Marks, 2019). Though previous literature suggests mixed possibilities on the association between EI and fraud tendency, the dearth of empirical research on fraud tendency and rationalization behaviors demands a critical response, especially given the rising occurrences of occupational fraud involving accountants, managers and directors of companies (Nanda et al., 2019; Rezaee and Riley, 2010; Sikka, 2017; Sikka and Lehman, 2015). While Gaspar et al. (2021) suggest a positive association between EI and fraud tendency, the current study expects a negative association in line with Marks' (2019) fraud pentagon model and the submissions of Rahmatullah et al. (2018), Ismail and Rasheed (2019) and Sulastri and Kasanah (2021). Thus, based on the suggestions of previous studies and the earlier theoretical discussion linking Ajzen's (1991) TPB with existing fraud models, the current study intends to contribute to the body of fraud literature by hypothesizing that EI and fraud tendency are negatively associated, suggesting that a high EI will lead to a low fraud tendency.

3. Research methods and data

3.1 Study design, population and sample size

This study adopts a survey strategy and uses a self-completed questionnaire to obtain primary data from the participants. The population consists of 493 registered final-year accounting students from three southwest Nigerian universities, made up of 210 students from the University of Lagos (UNILAG), representing federal universities, 220 students from the Lagos State University (LASU), representing state universities and 63 students from Afe Babalola University Ado-Ekiti (ABUAD), representing private universities, all sourced from the accounting departments of the respective universities. The study uses Yamane's (1967) sample formula to determine the sample size as 335 participants who are future accountants. The sample was drawn from the population using a random sampling design after grouping the participants into three strata according to their universities. Kothari and Garg (2014) note that random sampling is preferred to other techniques because it gives each individual equal chances to represent the population. It guarantees the Law of Statistical Regularity, which states that “if on an average the sample chosen is a random one, the sample will have the same composition and characteristics as the universe” (Kothari and Garg, 2014, p. 56).

3.2 Questionnaire design, validation and administration

The questionnaire design followed the suggestions of Oppenheim (1992) and Kothari and Garg (2014) on quality formatting, wording and question sequencing to enhance its validity and reliability and to limit social desirability and common-method biases. Face and content validities were certified by two associate chartered accountants and lecturers from two Nigerian universities. The study conducted a pilot test and used the feedback to revise the questionnaire before distribution. It also performed a Cronbach alpha (α) reliability test to assess the consistency of the composition parameter of the multiple-item scales used to measure the dependent (fraud tendency) and independent (EI) variables (Field, 2017) and how well each component measures the variables. Due to the coronavirus pandemic (COVID-19), the questionnaire was administered via an official online survey platform, the Joint Information Systems Committee (JISC) Online Survey Platform, recommended by the institution where the study was conducted. Besides, technological advances have made Internet-based surveys popular due to the wide range of benefits they offer, as they are cost-efficient, time-efficient, participant-friendly and quick to analyze compared to conventional methods. The JISC platform was secured and each participant required a password to access the survey within 14 days (July 23–August 6, 2021).

3.3 The measurement of variables

In testing its hypothesis, this study divides the measurement of variables into three categories, operationally defined as dependent, independent and control variables. First, the dependent variable, fraud tendency, is measured using a scenario that reflects a real-life fraud situation commonly found in developing countries with high-fraud profiles (See Appendix 1), and based on Ajzen's (1991) TPB, which states that the behaviors of individuals are guided and predicted based on their intentions, this study assumes the moral motivation of an individual reflects his\her behavioral tendency toward fraud (Ajzen, 1991; Kohlberg, 1973; Rest, 1982; Stone et al., 2009). The study holds that individuals who rationalize and justify a deceitful act based on surrounding situations or utilitarianism would engage in the same act when faced with similar situations or if they feel the end would justify the means (Ajzen and Fishbein, 2005; Cressey, 1953; Jones, 1991; Kohlberg, 1973; Marks, 2012, 2019; Wells, 2017). Accordingly, after the scenario, the participants were asked to respond to a construct containing four statements on two stages (moral judgment and moral intention) of Rest's (1979) EDM model on a five-point Likert scale (strongly agree – 5 to strongly disagree –1). The responses obtained from the four statements were aggregated and averaged into a single variable – fraud tendency.

Second, EI, the independent variable, is measured using Goleman's emotional intelligence questionnaire (EIQ), obtained from the National Health Service (NHS) leadership toolkit. The EIQ has 10 questionnaire items for each component of EI (self-awareness, self-regulation, motivation, empathy and social skill), amounting to 50 items used to measure EI as a single variable (see Appendix 1). Alexander and Becker (1978) and Weber (1992) noted that adopting previously validated measurement scales facilitates comparisons and cross-validation of results. Lastly, in an attempt to address any omitted variables-related issues (Wooldridge, 2016), this study employs a set of control variables, namely gender, academic intelligence, religion and income, that may impact individual behavioral tendencies and moral dispositions, chosen with reference to previous behavioral studies (Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen and Fishbein, 2005; Craft, 2013; Kohlberg, 1973; Lehnert et al., 2015; Oboh, 2019; Oboh and Omolehinwa, 2022). For example, Ajzen and Fishbein (2005) identified a wide variety of personal and situational factors, such as gender (male and female), age (young and old), education (educated and uneducated), and economic status (rich and poor), that may be influencing individual behavioral beliefs about a given behavior, leading to the formation of an intention or tendency towards the behavior.

To avoid or limit any potential common-method bias, as suggested by Change et al. (2010) and Podsakoff et al. (2003), the dependent and independent variables were measured using different measurement styles, and the order of questions was mixed. Also, the participants were asked to answer as honestly as possible, as there were no right or wrong answers, and were assured of the anonymity and confidentiality of the survey via a cover letter that had the official logo and address of the institution where the study was conducted.

3.4 Model specification

The study developed the following regression model to test its hypothesis and provide evidence that would explain the proposed association between EI and fraud tendency:

FTi=β0+β1EIi+β2GENi+β3AIi+β4RELi+β5INCi+εi
where FT is fraud tendency, EI is emotional intelligence, GEN is gender, AI is academic intelligence (measured by the current cumulative grade point average (CGPA) of the participants), REL is religion and INC is Income.

3.5 Data analysis methods

The data collected for analysis were checked for completeness, and there was no need to remove any responses. The sample size is adequate, with at least 15 cases per predictor (see Field, 2017). As Field (2017) suggests, the study used scatterplots of standardized predicted values vs standardized residuals to assess assumptions of normality, linearity and homoscedasticity. The variance inflation factor (VIF) showed no multicollinearity, and the Durbin–Watson test showed that errors were independent, meaning the regression model is free from autocorrelation. The study performed descriptive statistical analysis to assess the participants' demographic composition, EI and fraud tendency. It considers a mean score of ≥3 on a five-point Likert scale as high/agreement and <3 as low/disagreement. Also, the study performed Pearson correlation and regression analyses to test its hypothesis and establish an association between EI and fraud tendency.

3.5 Ethical considerations

Adherence to ethical regulations helps preserve the integrity of the research community. These regulations are principles researchers must observe to ensure that sensitive data in research are protected. Accordingly, this study obtained ethical approval to collect data after satisfying the guidelines of the institution where it was conducted. Every material from secondary sources is used with permission, or due acknowledgment has been given to the author(s), and to limit the bias of social desirability, demand characteristics and common-method variance, the participants were duly informed in a cover letter of the purpose of the study (academics) and that participation was voluntary. They were assured of the privacy of their responses as the questionnaire contained no identification mark and that by accepting to complete the survey, they were consenting to the use of their data in the research, but could retract their data if required.

4. Results

4.1 Preliminary analysis

The survey instrument was administered via an official online platform (JISC) to minimize the spread of COVID-19. Therefore, within the access period (14 days), 225 participants completed the questionnaire, the number of observations made within the period (see Table 1). The response rate (67%), on average, is considered adequate to achieve the study's objectives.

The alpha coefficients for the dependent and independent variables, presented in Table 2, are considered satisfactory as they certify that the questionnaire and measurement scales are reliable, stable and consistent (see Field, 2017; Peterson, 1994). Moreso, Nunnally (1978) and Prasongsukarn (2009) note that an alpha coefficient ranges between 0 and 1, and the closer to 1, the greater the scale's reliability.

The demographic composition of the participants is displayed in Table 3, which shows that female participants (66.7%) are more than male participants (33.3%), indicating a gender in-balance. The vast majority are aged 21-< 25 years. They are mostly Christians (74.7%) and Moslems (23.6%), the two dominant religions in Nigeria. About two-thirds (61.3%) are on first-class and second-class upper cumulative grade point average (CGPA), and their income levels varied, with the majority receiving above ₦20,000 (approximately $46.11, at ₦433.72 = $1, as at 06/10/2022) as monthly upkeep/income.

4.2 Descriptive analysis

As stated in the previous section, a mean score of ≥3 is considered high/agreement, while <3 is low/disagreement. Accordingly, Table 4 indicates that the participants have a high tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud (combined: mean = 3.49), as they rationalized and justified the fraud in the scenario. Equally, the participants display a high level of EI (combined: mean = 3.78).

4.3 Pearson correlation analysis

As displayed in Table 5, the results of the Pearson correlation did not support the hypothesis of the study. The results indicate that EI correlates positively with fraud tendency (p < 0.01), as against a negative association hypothesized. Among the control variables, academic intelligence correlated positively with fraud tendency (p < 0.01), but no significant correlation was found between gender, religion, income and fraud tendency (p > 0.05).

4.4 Regression analysis

Consequent to the data exploration results and diagnostics tests (See Appendix 2), the study performed a multiple linear regression analysis to establish an association between EI and fraud tendency. Field (2017) suggests linear regression is appropriate when the research goal is prediction or explanation. In addition, four control variables (gender, academic intelligence, religion and income) are included in the regression model to control the variability between EI and fraud tendency and enhance the internal validity. The result, reported in Table 6 with a total number of observations of 225 independent responses, indicates that EI and fraud tendency are positively associated (β = 0.328, p < 0.01). This result does not support the study's hypothesis since it hypothesized a negative relationship between these two variables. Among the control variables, academic intelligence associates with fraud tendency positively, which is significant at 0.01 (β = 0.185, p < 0.01), while gender, religion and income did not significantly associate with fraud tendency (p > 0.05). The model statistically accounts for about 13% (Adj. R2) variation in fraud tendency (F = 7.393, p < 0.01) as the t-test confirms that EI (t = 5.192, p < 0.01) and academic intelligence (t = 2.932, p < 0.01) made significant contributions in predicting the variation in fraud tendency, with EI contributing more than academic intelligence. The results of the diagnostic tests (average VIF = 1.023 and Durbin–Watson = 2.000) certified that the model is free from multicollinearity and auto-correlation problems.

In sum, based on the statistical evidence from the correlation (see Table 5) and regression (see Table 6) analyses, the study found statistical evidence that supports an association between EI and fraud tendency. But contrary to the negative ‘a priori’ expectation, the finding demonstrates that EI and fraud tendency are positively associated. In addition, the study found that academic intelligence, a control variable included in the regression model, positively associates with fraud tendency.

5. Discussion

This study examined the association between emotional intelligence and fraud tendency. The descriptive analysis results indicate that the participants (who are future accountants) exhibit a high level of EI, possessing the meta-level ability to understand and manage their emotions and those of others. This finding is similar to Ismail and Rasheed (2019), who found a high level of EI among future accountants in Malaysia. However, despite displaying a high level of EI, the findings indicate that future accountants in Nigeria have a high fraud tendency. They rationalized and justified the fraudulent act in the scenario because of the successes achieved, consenting that the end justifies the means in a fraud situation. They agree that it is not altogether wrong to engage in dishonest practices to achieve desired successes and would engage in such acts if they perceive the end would justify the means. In other words, they are willing to manipulate financial statements to evade tax or reduce their tax liabilities to advance their interest, as simulated in the fraud scenario. This finding aligns with Cressey (1953) on situational pressure, affirming that trustworthy people can betray trust when pressured to seek personal gratifications, a tendency displayed by future accountants in Nigeria, which should concern the counter fraud community, prospective employers, professional accounting bodies, the government and all stakeholders.

Contrary to the negative ‘a priori’ expectation, the study found statistical evidence from the correlation and regression analyses that support a positive association between EI and fraud tendency. Though EI significantly predicts fraud tendency, the positive association suggests that future accountants in Nigeria who are emotionally intelligent have a more heightened tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud. That is, fraud tendency is higher in emotionally intelligent individuals, despite having the ability to regulate and manage one's emotions and understand the effects of these emotions on others. While prior literature suggests EI makes a leader effective (Goleman, 1998; Bradbury and Graves, 2005), enhances his job performance (Law et al., 2008) and aids his cognitive processes in decision-making (Angelidis and Ibrahim, 2011; Ismail, 2015), this study suggests emotionally intelligent individuals have a high tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud. Though this finding sits at odds with Rahmatullah et al. (2018), who associated EQ with low fraud tendency, it agrees with Gaspar et al. (2021) that emotionally intelligent individuals have the highest likelihood to deceive or act fraudulently. One reason for the high fraud tendency among future accountants in Nigeria may be a result of what Okunade and Shehu (2015) and Ibrahim and Bukar (2021) describe as the “get-rich-quick syndrome” among many young Nigerians. Besides, the ACFE (2020) confirmed that occupational fraud is on the rise globally, but the fraud profile in developing countries is worrying (Ajibolade and Oboh, 2017; Oboh and Ajibolade, 2018; Ogunleye, 2015; Transparency International, 2020), which may account for the high fraud tendency among future accountants in Nigeria.

Furthermore, of the four control variables included in the regression model to address any omitted variables-related issues, academic intelligence alone positively associates with fraud tendency. Gender, religion and income did not significantly associate with the fraud tendency of the participants. The positive association between academic intelligence and fraud tendency suggests that the tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud is high in academically intelligent individuals. This finding aligns with the arguments of Wolfe and Hermanson (2004) and Marks (2019) that fraud perpetrators possess some intelligence and ability to diagnose and exploit fraud opportunities and make them a reality. Thus, from the evidence in this study, it is logical to argue that fraud rationalization and tendency are higher in intellectually intelligent individuals, which enables them to scheme, commit and conceal a fraud. That is, it requires some intellectual ability to manipulate internal control systems, rationalize, invent cover-up schemes and exploit fraud opportunities in organizations, especially financial statement fraud which requires a high level of intellectual intelligence to execute successfully. Though this finding adds to the literature, more empirical studies are needed to establish a strong tie between academic intelligence and the tendency of individuals to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud.

6. Conclusion and implications for theory and practice

The independent associations of EI and fraud tendency with other variables have been studied previously. But this study directly associates these two inherent behavioral factors together, being the first to do so from a developing country. Though contrary to the negative a priori expectation, the study found a positive association between the two variables (EI and fraud tendency), which constitutes the main contribution of this study. The positive association between the variables suggests that future accountants in Nigeria that are emotionally intelligent have a high tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud. Thus, fraud theorists and behavioral psychologists would find the study helpful in recommending counter fraud interventions to limit rationalization behaviors among accountants and, by extension, other professionals.

Also, given that previous studies of fraud tendency and rationalization behavior towards occupational fraud have focused on developed countries, focusing on future accountants in Nigeria adds significantly to the empirical evidence regarding the developing world, which is the second contribution of this study. This contribution is of particular significance considering the research topic because Nigeria represents developing countries with high-fraud profiles and underdeveloped counter fraud strategies (Bakre, 2007; Oboh, 2019; The Central Bank of Nigeria (CBN), 2017; The Nigeria Inter-Bank Settlement System (NIBSS), 2020; Transparency International, 2020). And so, the study would benefit the counter fraud community in Nigeria and serve as a benchmark for future research in other developing countries with similar fraud profiles. Likewise, previous studies on occupational fraud were limited to external factors focusing on managers, directors and other employees of companies, but the study of future accountants who are potential preparers of financial statements, auditors, tax consultants and management accountants offers rare insights into the fraud tendency of these aspiring professionals. Thus, recruiters, employers and human resource managers will find the study beneficial in decision-making relating to job recruitment, placements and moral orientation for prospective accountant employees.

As part of its contributions, this study found a direct association between academic intelligence and fraud tendency, which is positive, suggesting that the tendency to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud is high in academically intelligent individuals. Thus, the study complements previous studies from developed countries, especially the studies of Wolfe and Hermanson (2004) and Marks (2019) that introduced capability and competence as fraud factors that enable perpetrators to diagnose, exploit opportunities to commit fraud, and conceal them.

Finally, this study combines two stages (moral judgment and moral intention) of Rest's (1979) EDM model and applies Cressey's (1953) fraud triangle to create a scenario and scale that measures fraud tendency. It performed a Cronbach alpha test to estimate the consistency of the measurement composition parameters and how well each questionnaire item measures the variables. The outcome of the Cronbach tests, which is adequate, suggests that future researchers can adopt the scenario and scale to measure individual fraud tendencies, as they reflect a real-life fraud situation commonly found in developing countries with high-fraud profiles. In addition, as suggested by psychometricians, no one measure is regarded entirely as valid in every context, but measurement scales undergo continuous validation processes (Flory et al., 1992; Meadows, 2003), and the measures of potentially broad construct, such as EI and fraud tendency, must be subjected to tests in various settings and with different groups to confirm that the initial results are not artifacts of a particular group or sceneries. Thus, using the EIQ in this study further validates the measurement scales obtained from the NHS leadership toolkit.

7. Limitations and suggestions for future research

This study has achieved its objectives by providing empirical evidence on the direct association between EI and the tendency of future accountants to rationalize and engage in occupational fraud. Nevertheless, it acknowledges that since it is among the first to associate fraud tendency with EI, further studies will be needed to corroborate its finding. Due to the fact that the study is performed in a specific country and on specific respondents, a caveat should be observed when generalizing its findings. Because of its limitations, there are other ways to build on the current study. First, future research can advance knowledge on fraud tendency by using a mix of questionnaires and interviews to collect data to allow the participants to share their experiences with fraud. Second, future studies can adopt and validate the scenario and scale developed in this study to measure individual fraud tendencies in other countries. Finally, future research will benefit from a larger sample, as the COVID-19 pandemic limited the data collection in this study.

Figures

Test of normality, homogeneity and linearity - emotional intelligence and fraud tendency

Figure A1

Test of normality, homogeneity and linearity - emotional intelligence and fraud tendency

Sample size determination and questionnaire response rate

InstitutionPopulationSample (N/(1 + Neˆ2))Responses/ObservationsPercentage
University of Lagos21013812591
Lagos State University2201425639
Afe Babalola University63554480
Total49333522567

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Reliability test: fraud tendency (Cronbach alpha)

MeasuresQuestionnaire itemsAlpha (α)
Fraud tendency (FT)40.90
Emotional intelligence (EI)500.93
Sub-components:
Self-awareness100.74
Self-regulation100.68
Motivation100.81
Empathy100.77
Social skill100.80

Note(s): Number of observations = 225

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Participants demography

Variables Frequency (%)
GenderMale75 (33.3)
Female150 (66.7)
Age BracketBelow 21 years33 (14.7)
21-< 25 years173 (76.9)
25–30 years19 (8.4)
ReligionChristianity168 (74.7)
Islam53 (23.6)
Traditionalist1 (0.4)
Atheist1 (0.4)
Others2 (0.9)
Cumulative Grade Point Average (CGPA)1.50–2.4917 (7.6)
2.50–3.4970 (31.1)
3.50–4.49115 (51.1)
4.50–5.0023 (10.2)
Monthly Upkeep/IncomeLess than ₦5,000/month20 (8.9)
₦5,000 -<₦10,000/month40 (17.8)
₦10,000 - ₦20,000/month67 (29.8)
Above ₦20,000/month98 (43.6)

Note(s): Number of observations (N) = 225

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Descriptive analysis: fraud tendency and emotional intelligence

MeanStandard deviationSum statisticMin. statisticMax. statistic
Dependent var.: Fraud tendency (FT) (combined)3.491.0927851.005.00
Q1: Akin Peters is not altogether wrong in his decision since it is to advance himself in life3.431.2777721.005.00
Q2: Occasionally, it is not totally wrong to make such decisions to achieve greater heights in life3.481.2367821.005.00
Q3: Akin Peters' successes justify the decision he made3.511.1777901.005.00
Q4: If I were Akin Peters, I would make the same decision to achieve greater heights in life3.531.2827951.005.00
Independent var.: Emotional intelligence (EI) (combined)3.780.440852.382.405.00
Self-awareness4.050.454910.802.705.00
Self-regulation3.570.533804.302.005.00
Motivation3.680.589829.101.705.00
Empathy3.870.487870.202.305.00
Social skill3.770.576847.501.305.00

Note(s): Number of observations = 225

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Pearson correlation matrix

FTEIGENAIRELINC
FTPearson correlation1
Sig. (2-tailed)
N225
EIPearson correlation0.330**1
Sig. (2-tailed)0.000
N225225
GENPearson correlation0.0710.1121
Sig. (2-tailed)0.2910.093
N225225225
AIPearson correlation0.181**0.001−0.0371
Sig. (2-tailed)0.0060.9840.581
N225225225225
RELPearson correlation−0.025−0.0240.112−0.0911
Sig. (2-tailed)0.7140.7230.0950.175
N225225225225225
INCPearson correlation0.0150.082−0.0290.109−0.0251
Sig. (2-tailed)0.8270.2180.6670.1020.708
N225225225225225225

Note(s): **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). Number of observations = 225

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Regression analysis: FTi=β0+β1EIi+β2GENi+β3AIi+β4RELi+β5INCi+εi

Model summary
ModelRR squareAdjusted R squareStd. Error of the estimateDurbin–Watson
10.3800.1440.1251.021632.000
ANOVA
ModelSum of squaresdfMean squareFSig
1Regression38.57957.7167.393**0.000
Residual228.5752191.044
Total267.154224
Coefficients
ModelUnstandardized coefficientsStandardized coefficients SigCollinearity statistics
BStd. ErrorBetatToleranceVIF
1(Constant)−0.2290.859 −0.2670.790
EI0.8140.1570.328**5.192**0.0000.9791.022
GEN0.0930.1470.0400.6360.5250.9731.028
AI0.2640.0900.185**2.932**0.0040.9801.021
REL−0.0100.116−0.005−0.0830.9340.9791.022
INC−0.0350.070−0.032−0.5000.6170.9801.020
Average VIF = 1.023

Note(s): a. Dependent Variable: FT; **significant at 0.01. Number of observations = 225

Source(s): Author's computation (2023) from SPSS version 25

Appendix 1 Research instrument – questionnaire

Appendix 2

Figure A1

References

Adekoya, A.C., Oboh, C.S. and Oyewumi, O.R. (2020), “Accountants perception of the factors influencing auditors' ethical behavior in Nigeria”, Heliyon, Vol. 6 No. 6, e04271, doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04271.

Ajekwe, C.C. and Ibiamke, A. (2017), “Accounting frauds: a review of literature”, IOSR Journal of Humanities and Social Science, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 42-43.

Ajibolade, S.O. and Oboh, C.S. (2017), “A critical examination of government budgeting and public funds management in Nigeria”, International Journal of Public Leadership, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 218-242.

Ajzen, I. (1991), “The theory of planned behavior”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 50 No. 2, pp. 179-211.

Ajzen, I. (2012), “Martin Fishbein's legacy: the reasoned action approach”, The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 640, pp. 11-27.

Ajzen, I. and Fishbein, M. (2005), “The influence of attitudes on behavior”, in Albarracín, D., Johnson, B.T. and Zanna, M.P. (Eds), The Handbook of Attitudes, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.

Ajzen, I. and Madden, T.J. (1986), “Prediction of goal-directed behavior: attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioral control”, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 22, pp. 453-474.

Albrecht, W.S., Albrecht, C.O., Albrecht, C.C. and Zimbelman, M.F. (2019), Fraud Examination, 6th ed., Cengage.

Alexander, C.S. and Becker, H.J. (1978), “The use of vignettes in survey research”, Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 93-104.

Andon, P., Free, C. and Scard, B. (2015), “Pathways to accountant fraud: Australian evidence and analysis”, Accounting Research Journal, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 10-44.

Angelidis, J. and Ibrahim, N.A. (2011), “The impact of emotional intelligence on the ethical judgment of managers”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 99 No. 1, pp. 111-119.

Armstrong, M.B., Ketz, J.E. and Owsen, D. (2003), “Ethics education in accounting: moving toward ethical motivation and ethical behaviors”, Journal of Accounting Education, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 1-16.

Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE) (2020), “Report to the Nations: 2020 Global study on occupational fraud and abuse”, available at: https://acfepublic.s3-us-west-2.amazonaws.com/2020-Report-to-the-Nations.pdf (accessed 5 March 2021).

Awang, Y. and Ismail, S. (2018), “Determinants of financial reporting fraud intention among accounting practitioners in the banking sector”, International Journal of Ethics and Systems, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 32-54, doi: 10.1108/IJOES-05-2017-0080.

Awang, Y., Rahman, A.R.A. and Ismail, S. (2019), “The influences of attitude, subjective norm and adherence to Islamic professional ethics on fraud intention in financial reporting”, Journal of Islamic Accounting and Business Research, Vol. 10 No. 5, pp. 710-725.

Bakre, O.M. (2007), “The unethical practices of accountants and auditors and the compromising stance of professional bodies in the corporate world: evidence from Corporate Nigeria”, Accounting Forum, Vol. 31 No. 3, pp. 277-303.

Baucus, M.S. (1994), “Pressure, opportunity and predisposition: a multivariate model of corporate illegality”, Journal of Management, Vol. 20 No. 4, pp. 699-721.

Bradbury, T. and Graves, J. (2005), Emotional Intelligence: Skills and Tests, Savalan Publisher, Tehran.

Change, S., Witteloostuijn, A.V. and Eden, L. (2010), “From the editors: common method variance in international research”, Journal of International Business Studies, Vol. 41, pp. 178-184, doi: 10.1057/jibs.2009.88.

CIA (2017), “The world factbook”, Central Intelligence Agency, available at: www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook (assessed 23 April 2017).

Craft, J.L. (2013), “A review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: 2004-2011”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 117 No. 2, pp. 221-259.

Cressey, D.R. (1953), Other People's Money; a Study of the Social Psychology of Embezzlement, Free Press.

Deshpande, S.P. and Joseph, J. (2009), “Impact of emotional intelligence, ethical climate, and behavior of peers on ethical behavior of nurses”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 85 No. 3, pp. 403-410.

Dewi, K. and Anisykurlillah, I. (2021), “Analysis of the effect of fraud pentagon factors on fraudulent financial statement with audit committee as moderating variable”, Accounting Analysis Journal, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 39-46.

Doig, A. (2012), Fraud: The Counter Fraud Practitioner's Handbook, 1st ed., Routledge - Taylor & Francis Group, London.

Dorminey, J., Fleming, A.S., Kranacher, M.J. and Riley, R.A. Jr (2012), “The evolution of fraud theory”, Issues in Accounting Education, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 555-579.

Dulewicz, V. and Higgs, M. (2005), “Assessing leadership styles and organizational context”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 105-123.

Field, A.P. (2017), Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th ed., SAGE, London.

Flory, S.M., Phillips, T.J., Reidenbach, R.E. and Robin, D.P. (1992), “A multidimensional analysis of selected ethical issues in accounting”, Accounting Review, Vol. 67 No. 2, pp. 284-302.

Forsyth, D.R. (1980), “Taxonomy of ethical ideologies”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 175-184.

Gaspar, J.P., Methasani, R. and Schweitzer, M.E. (2021), “Emotional intelligence and deception: a theoretical model and propositions”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 177 No. 3, pp. 567-584, doi: 10.1007/s10551-021-04738-y.

Gómez-Leal, R., Holzer, A.A., Bradley, C., Fernández-Berrocal, P. and Patti, J. (2022), “The relationship between emotional intelligence and leadership in school leaders: a systematic review”, Cambridge Journal of Education, Vol. 52 No. 1, pp. 1-21, doi: 10.1080/0305764X.2021.1927987.

Goleman, D. (1998), “What makes a leader?”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 76, pp. 92-105.

Gransberry, C.K. (2022), “How emotional intelligence promotes leadership and management practices”, Public Organization Review, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 935-948.

Griffin, O. (2015), “Unethical practices rife among accountants”, Economia, available at: https://economia.icaew.com/news/november-2015/unethical-practices-rife-among-accountants (assessed 3 June 2017).

Holtfreter, K. (2005), “Is occupational fraud ‘typical’ white-collar crime? A comparison of individual and organizational characteristics”, Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 33 No. 4, pp. 353-365, doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2005.04.005.

Ibrahim, S.Y. and Bukar, H.M. (2021), “Thickly populated and abundant resources but struggling with development: the Case of Nigeria”, Theoretical Economics Letters, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 643-647.

Ismail, S. (2015), “Influence of emotional intelligence, ethical climates, and corporate ethical values on ethical judgment of Malaysian auditors”, Asian Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 147-162.

Ismail, S. and Rasheed, Z. (2019), “Influence of ethical ideology and emotional intelligence on the ethical judgement of future accountants in Malaysia”, Meditari Accountancy Research, Vol. 27 No. 6, pp. 805-822, doi: 10.1108/MEDAR-04-2018-0326.

Jones, T.M. (1991), “Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: an issue-contingent model”, The Academy of Management Review, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 366-395.

Keefer, K.V., Parker, J.D. and Saklofske, D.H. (2018), “Three decades of emotional intelligence research: perennial issues, emerging trends, and lessons learned in education: introduction to emotional intelligence in education”, Emotional Intelligence in Education, Springer, Cham, pp. 1-19.

Kohlberg, L. (1973), “The claim to moral adequacy of a highest stage of moral judgment”, The Journal of Philosophy, Vol. 70 No. 18, pp. 630-646.

Kothari, C.R. and Garg, G. (2014), Research Methodology: Methods and Techniques, 3rd ed., New Age International (P) Limited Publishers, New Delhi.

Law, K.S., Wong, C.S., Huang, G.H. and Li, X. (2008), “The effects of emotional intelligence on job performance and life satisfaction for the research and development scientists in China”, Asia Pacific Journal of Management, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 51-69.

Lehnert, K., Park, Y. and Singh, N. (2015), “Research note and review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: boundary conditions and extensions”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 129 No. 1, pp. 195-219.

Lone, M.A. and Lone, A.H. (2018), “Does emotional intelligence predict leadership effectiveness? An exploration in non-Western context”, South Asian Journal of Human Resources Management, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 28-39, doi: 10.1177/2322093718766806.

Mandell, B. and Pherwani, S. (2003), “Relationship between emotional intelligence and transformational leadership style: a gender comparison”, Journal of Business and Psychology, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 387-404.

Marks, J. (2012), “The mind behind the fraudsters crime: key behavioral and environmental elements”, Crowe Howarth LLP (Presentation), available at: https://www.fraudconference.com/uploadedFiles/Fraud_Conference/Content/Course-Materials/presentations/23rd/ppt/10C-Jonathan-Marks.pdf (accessed 2 April 2021).

Marks, J.T. (2019), “Fraud pentagon – enhancements to the three conditions under which fraud may occur”, Board and Fraud, available at: https://boardandfraud.com/2020/05/21/fraud-pentagon-enhancements-to-the-fraud-triangle-and-under-which-fraud-may-occur/ (accessed 2 April 2021).

Matthews, C. and Gandel, S. (2015), “The 5 biggest corporate scandals of 2015”, The Fortune, available at: http://fortune.com

Meadows, K.A. (2003), “So you want to do research? 4: an introduction to quantitative methods”, British Journal of Community Nursing, Vol. 8 No. 11, pp. 519-526.

Mesmer-Magnus, J., Viswesvaran, C., Deshpande, S.P. and Joseph, J. (2010), “Emotional intelligence, individual ethicality, and perceptions that unethical behavior facilitates success”, Revista de Psicología del Trabajo y de las Organizaciones, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 35-45.

Nanda, S.T., Zenita, R. and Salmiah, N. (2019), “Fraudulent financial reporting: a fraud pentagon analysis”, Accounting and Finance Review, Vol. 4 No. 4, pp. 106-113.

Nunnally, J.C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, McGraw-Hill, New York.

Oboh, C.S. (2019), “Personal and moral intensity determinants of ethical decision-making: a study of accounting professionals in Nigeria”, Journal of Accounting in Emerging Economies, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 148-180.

Oboh, C.S. and Ajibolade, S.O. (2018), “Personal characteristics and ethical decision-making process of accounting professionals in Nigeria”, Crawford Journal of Business and Social Sciences, Vol. 8 No. 1, pp. 1-23.

Oboh, C.S. and Omolehinwa, E.O. (2022), “Sociodemographic variables and ethical decision-making: a survey of professional accountants in Nigeria”, RAUSP Management Journal, Vol. 57 No. 2, pp. 131-148, doi: 10.1108/RAUSP-04-2020-0086.

Oboh, C.S., Ajibolade, S.O. and Otusanya, O.J. (2020), “Ethical decision-making among professional accountants in Nigeria: the influence of ethical ideology, work sector, and types of professional membership”, Journal of Financial Reporting and Accounting, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 389-422, doi: 10.1108/JFRA-09-2019-0123.

Ogunleye, O.J. (2015), “Accounting ethics and financial practices of accountants in Nigeria”, PhD Dissertation, Obafemi Awolowo University, Ile-Ife, Osun.

Okunade, S.K. and Shehu, H. (2015), “Get rich quick syndrome and Nigerian literature in the 21st Century: an examination of Ameh's Sweet Taste of Shame”, International Journal of Linguistics, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 47-51, doi: 10.15640/ijlc.v3n2a5.

Oppenheim, A.N. (1992), Questionnaire Design, Interviewing, and Attitude Measurement, New edition, Pinter Publishers, London.

Peterson, R.A. (1994), “A meta-analysis of Cronbach's coefficient alpha”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 381-391.

Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.Y. and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), “Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 5, pp. 879-903, doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.

Prasongsukarn, K. (2009), “Validating the cultural value scale (CVSCALE): a case study of Thailand”, ABAC Journal, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 1-13.

Rahmatullah, N., Irianto, G., Rosidi and Rafiqoh (2018), “The effects of intelligence quotient, emotional quotient, and spiritual quotient on the tendency of accounting fraud”, International Journal of Scientific and Technology Research, Vol. 7 No. 9, pp. 212-216.

Ratmono, D. and Frendy (2022), “Examining the fraud diamond theory through ethical culture variables: a study of regional development banks in Indonesia”, Cogent Business and Management, Vol. 9 No. 1, 2117161, doi: 10.1080/23311975.2022.2117161.

Rest, J. (1979), Development in Judging Moral Issues, University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis.

Rest, J. (1982), “A psychologist looks at the teaching of ethics”, The Hastings Center Report, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 29-36.

Rezaee, Z. (2005), “Causes, consequences, and deterrence of financial statement fraud”, Critical Perspectives on Accounting, Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 277-298.

Rezaee, Z. and Riley, R. (2010), Financial Statement Fraud: Prevention and Detection, 2nd ed., John.

Rufus, R.J., Miller, L.S. and Hahn, W. (2015), Forensic Accounting, Global edition, Pearson Education.

Schlaegel, C., Engle, R.L. and Lang, G. (2022), “The unique and common effects of emotional intelligence dimensions on job satisfaction and facets of job performance: an exploratory study in three countries”, The International Journal of Human Resource Management, Vol. 33 No. 8, pp. 1562-1605, doi: 10.1080/09585192.2020.1811368.

Schwartz, M.S. (2016), “Ethical decision-making theory: an integrated approach”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 139 No. 4, pp. 755-776.

Sikka, P. (2009), “Financial crisis and the silence of the auditors”, Accounting, Organizations and Society, Vol. 34 No. 6, pp. 868-873.

Sikka, P. (2017), “Accounting and taxation: conjoined twins or separate siblings?”, Accounting Forum, Vol. 41 No. 4, pp. 390-405.

Sikka, P. and Lehman, G. (2015), “The supply-side of corruption and limits to preventing corruption within government procurement and constructing ethical subjects”, Critical Perspectives on Accounting, Vol. 28, pp. 62-70.

Stone, T.H., Jawahar, I.M. and Kisamore, J.L. (2009), “Using the theory of planned behavior and cheating justifications to predict academic misconduct”, Career Development International, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 221-241, doi: 10.1108/13620430910966415.

Sulastri, S. and Kasanah, I.U. (2021), “Does emotional intelligence and religiosity affect the ethical judgment of accounting students?”, Journal of Accounting and Business Education, Vol. 5 No. 2, pp. 21-30.

Sy, T., Tram, S. and O’hara, L.A. (2006), “Relation of employee and manager emotional intelligence to job satisfaction and performance”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 68 No. 3, pp. 461-473.

The Central Bank of Nigeria (CBN) (2017), “The Nigeria electronic fraud forum annual reports 2016”, available at: https://www.cbn.gov.ng/Out/2017/CCD/A%20CHANGING%20PAYMENTS%20ECOSYSTEM%20NeFF%202016%20Annual%20Report.pdf

The Nigeria Inter-Bank Settlement System (NIBSS) (2020), “Fraud in the Nigerian financial services”, available at: https://nibss-plc.com.ng/media/PDFs/post/NIBSS%20Insights%20Fraud.pdf

The Nigerian Criminal Code Act (2004), Laws of the Federation of Nigeria, 2004, Cityway Publications.

The United Kingdom Fraud Act (2006), (Commencement) Order 2006, SI 2006/3200 (C 112), available at: https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/2006/35/pdfs/ukpgaen_20060035_en.pdf (accessed 5 March 2021).

Transparency International (2020), “Corruption perceptions index”, available at: http://www.transparency.org

Uche, C.U. (2002), “Professional accounting development in Nigeria: threats from the inside and outside”, Accounting, Organization and Society, Vol. 27 Nos 4/5, pp. 471-496.

United Nations Development Programme (UNDP) (2017), “The UNDP policy against fraud and other corrupt practices”, available at: https://popp.undp.org/_layouts/15/WopiFrame.aspx?sourcedoc=/UNDP_POPP_DOCUMENT_LIBRARY/Public/AC_Internal%20Control%20Framework_Anti-Fraud%20Policy.docx&action=default (accessed 15 March 2021).

van Rooy, D.L. and Viswesvaran, C. (2004), “Emotional intelligence: a meta-analytic investigation of predictive validity and nomological net”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 65 No. 1, pp. 71-95.

Vousinas, G.L. (2019), “Advancing theory of fraud: the SCORE model”, Journal of Financial Crime, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 372-381, doi: 10.1108/JFC-12-2017-0128.

Weber, J. (1992), “Vignettes in business ethics research: review, critical assessment, and recommendations”, Business Ethics Quarterly, Vol. 2 No. 2, pp. 137-160.

Wells, J.T. (2017), Corporate Fraud Handbook: Prevention and Detection, 5th ed., John Wiley & Sons, NJ.

Wolfe, D.T. and Hermanson, D.R. (2004), “The fraud diamond: considering the four elements of fraud”, CPA Journal, Vol. 74 No. 12, pp. 38-42.

Wong, C.S. and Law, K.S. (2002), “The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: an exploratory study”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 243-274.

Wood, H., Keatinge, T., Ditcham, K. and Janjeva, A. (2021), “The silent threat: the impact of fraud on UK national security”, RUSI Occasional Paper, January, pp. 1-56.

Wooldridge, J.M. (2016), Introductory Econometrics: A Modern Approach, 6th ed., Nelson Education, Toronto.

Yamane, T. (1967), Statistics: an Introductory Analysis, 2nd ed., Harper and Row Publishers, New York, NY.

Yang, L., Brink, A.G. and Wier, B. (2018), “The impact of emotional intelligence on auditor judgment”, International Journal of Auditing, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 83-97.

Further reading

van Akkeren, J. and Buckby, S. (2017), “Perceptions on the causes of individual and fraudulent co-offending: views of forensic accountants”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 146 No. 2, pp. 383-404.

Acknowledgements

Funding: This research is funded by the Foreign, Commonwealth and Development Office and partner organisations, United Kingdom, through the prestigious Chevening Scholarship Award.

Corresponding author

Collins Sankay Oboh can be contacted at: coboh@unilag.edu.ng

Related articles